- Dutch
- Frisian
- Saterfrisian
- Afrikaans
-
- Phonology
- Segment inventory
- Phonotactics
- Phonological processes
- Phonology-morphology interface
- Word stress
- Primary stress in simplex words
- Monomorphemic words
- Diachronic aspects
- Generalizations on stress placement
- Default penultimate stress
- Lexical stress
- The closed penult restriction
- Final closed syllables
- The diphthong restriction
- Superheavy syllables (SHS)
- The three-syllable window
- Segmental restrictions
- Phonetic correlates
- Stress shifts in loanwords
- Quantity-sensitivity
- Secondary stress
- Vowel reduction in unstressed syllables
- Stress in complex words
- Primary stress in simplex words
- Accent & intonation
- Clitics
- Spelling
- Morphology
- Word formation
- Compounding
- Nominal compounds
- Verbal compounds
- Adjectival compounds
- Affixoids
- Coordinative compounds
- Synthetic compounds
- Reduplicative compounds
- Phrase-based compounds
- Elative compounds
- Exocentric compounds
- Linking elements
- Separable complex verbs (SCVs)
- Gapping of complex words
- Particle verbs
- Copulative compounds
- Derivation
- Numerals
- Derivation: inputs and input restrictions
- The meaning of affixes
- Non-native morphology
- Cohering and non-cohering affixes
- Prefixation
- Suffixation
- Nominal suffixation: person nouns
- Conversion
- Pseudo-participles
- Bound forms
- Nouns
- Nominal prefixes
- Nominal suffixes
- -aal and -eel
- -aar
- -aard
- -aat
- -air
- -aris
- -ast
- Diminutives
- -dom
- -een
- -ees
- -el (nominal)
- -elaar
- -enis
- -er (nominal)
- -erd
- -erik
- -es
- -eur
- -euse
- ge...te
- -heid
- -iaan, -aan
- -ief
- -iek
- -ier
- -ier (French)
- -ière
- -iet
- -igheid
- -ij and allomorphs
- -ijn
- -in
- -ing
- -isme
- -ist
- -iteit
- -ling
- -oir
- -oot
- -rice
- -schap
- -schap (de)
- -schap (het)
- -sel
- -st
- -ster
- -t
- -tal
- -te
- -voud
- Verbs
- Adjectives
- Adverbs
- Univerbation
- Neo-classical word formation
- Construction-dependent morphology
- Morphological productivity
- Compounding
- Inflection
- Inflection and derivation
- Allomorphy
- The interface between phonology and morphology
- Word formation
- Syntax
- Preface and acknowledgements
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
- 1 Characterization and classification
- 2 Projection of verb phrases I:Argument structure
- 3 Projection of verb phrases II:Verb frame alternations
- Introduction
- 3.1. Main types
- 3.2. Alternations involving the external argument
- 3.3. Alternations of noun phrases and PPs
- 3.3.1. Dative/PP alternations (dative shift)
- 3.3.1.1. Dative alternation with aan-phrases (recipients)
- 3.3.1.2. Dative alternation with naar-phrases (goals)
- 3.3.1.3. Dative alternation with van-phrases (sources)
- 3.3.1.4. Dative alternation with bij-phrases (possessors)
- 3.3.1.5. Dative alternation with voor-phrases (benefactives)
- 3.3.1.6. Conclusion
- 3.3.1.7. Bibliographical notes
- 3.3.2. Accusative/PP alternations
- 3.3.3. Nominative/PP alternations
- 3.3.1. Dative/PP alternations (dative shift)
- 3.4. Some apparent cases of verb frame alternation
- 3.5. Bibliographical notes
- 4 Projection of verb phrases IIIa:Selection of clauses/verb phrases
- 5 Projection of verb phrases IIIb:Argument and complementive clauses
- Introduction
- 5.1. Finite argument clauses
- 5.2. Infinitival argument clauses
- 5.3. Complementive clauses
- 6 Projection of verb phrases IIIc:Complements of non-main verbs
- 7 Projection of verb phrases IIId:Verb clusters
- 8 Projection of verb phrases IV: Adverbial modification
- 9 Word order in the clause I:General introduction
- 10 Word order in the clause II:Position of the finite verb (verb-first/second)
- 11 Word order in the clause III:Clause-initial position (wh-movement)
- Introduction
- 11.1. The formation of V1- and V2-clauses
- 11.2. Clause-initial position remains (phonetically) empty
- 11.3. Clause-initial position is filled
- 12 Word order in the clause IV:Postverbal field (extraposition)
- 13 Word order in the clause V: Middle field (scrambling)
- 14 Main-clause external elements
- Nouns and Noun Phrases
- 1 Characterization and classification
- 2 Projection of noun phrases I: complementation
- Introduction
- 2.1. General observations
- 2.2. Prepositional and nominal complements
- 2.3. Clausal complements
- 2.4. Bibliographical notes
- 3 Projection of noun phrases II: modification
- Introduction
- 3.1. Restrictive and non-restrictive modifiers
- 3.2. Premodification
- 3.3. Postmodification
- 3.3.1. Adpositional phrases
- 3.3.2. Relative clauses
- 3.3.3. Infinitival clauses
- 3.3.4. A special case: clauses referring to a proposition
- 3.3.5. Adjectival phrases
- 3.3.6. Adverbial postmodification
- 3.4. Bibliographical notes
- 4 Projection of noun phrases III: binominal constructions
- Introduction
- 4.1. Binominal constructions without a preposition
- 4.2. Binominal constructions with a preposition
- 4.3. Bibliographical notes
- 5 Determiners: articles and pronouns
- Introduction
- 5.1. Articles
- 5.2. Pronouns
- 5.3. Bibliographical notes
- 6 Numerals and quantifiers
- 7 Pre-determiners
- Introduction
- 7.1. The universal quantifier al 'all' and its alternants
- 7.2. The pre-determiner heel 'all/whole'
- 7.3. A note on focus particles
- 7.4. Bibliographical notes
- 8 Syntactic uses of noun phrases
- Adjectives and Adjective Phrases
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- 2 Projection of adjective phrases I: Complementation
- 3 Projection of adjective phrases II: Modification
- 4 Projection of adjective phrases III: Comparison
- 5 Attributive use of the adjective phrase
- 6 Predicative use of the adjective phrase
- 7 The partitive genitive construction
- 8 Adverbial use of the adjective phrase
- 9 Participles and infinitives: their adjectival use
- 10 Special constructions
- Adpositions and adpositional phrases
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- Introduction
- 1.1. Characterization of the category adposition
- 1.2. A formal classification of adpositional phrases
- 1.3. A semantic classification of adpositional phrases
- 1.3.1. Spatial adpositions
- 1.3.2. Temporal adpositions
- 1.3.3. Non-spatial/temporal prepositions
- 1.4. Borderline cases
- 1.5. Bibliographical notes
- 2 Projection of adpositional phrases: Complementation
- 3 Projection of adpositional phrases: Modification
- 4 Syntactic uses of the adpositional phrase
- 5 R-pronominalization and R-words
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- Phonology
-
- General
- Phonology
- Segment inventory
- Phonotactics
- Phonological Processes
- Assimilation
- Vowel nasalization
- Syllabic sonorants
- Final devoicing
- Fake geminates
- Vowel hiatus resolution
- Vowel reduction introduction
- Schwa deletion
- Schwa insertion
- /r/-deletion
- d-insertion
- {s/z}-insertion
- t-deletion
- Intrusive stop formation
- Breaking
- Vowel shortening
- h-deletion
- Replacement of the glide w
- Word stress
- Clitics
- Allomorphy
- Orthography of Frisian
- Morphology
- Inflection
- Word formation
- Derivation
- Prefixation
- Infixation
- Suffixation
- Nominal suffixes
- Verbal suffixes
- Adjectival suffixes
- Adverbial suffixes
- Numeral suffixes
- Interjectional suffixes
- Onomastic suffixes
- Conversion
- Compositions
- Derivation
- Syntax
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Unergative and unaccusative subjects
- Evidentiality
- To-infinitival clauses
- Predication and noun incorporation
- Ellipsis
- Imperativus-pro-Infinitivo
- Expression of irrealis
- Embedded Verb Second
- Agreement
- Negation
- Nouns & Noun Phrases
- Classification
- Complementation
- Modification
- Partitive noun constructions
- Referential partitive constructions
- Partitive measure nouns
- Numeral partitive constructions
- Partitive question constructions
- Nominalised quantifiers
- Kind partitives
- Partitive predication with prepositions
- Bare nominal attributions
- Articles and names
- Pronouns
- Quantifiers and (pre)determiners
- Interrogative pronouns
- R-pronouns
- Syntactic uses
- Adjective Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Complementation
- Modification and degree quantification
- Comparison by degree
- Comparative
- Superlative
- Equative
- Attribution
- Agreement
- Attributive adjectives vs. prenominal elements
- Complex adjectives
- Noun ellipsis
- Co-occurring adjectives
- Predication
- Partitive adjective constructions
- Adverbial use
- Participles and infinitives
- Adposition Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Complementation
- Modification
- Intransitive adpositions
- Predication
- Preposition stranding
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
-
- General
- Morphology
- Morphology
- 1 Word formation
- 1.1 Compounding
- 1.1.1 Compounds and their heads
- 1.1.2 Special types of compounds
- 1.1.2.1 Affixoids
- 1.1.2.2 Coordinative compounds
- 1.1.2.3 Synthetic compounds and complex pseudo-participles
- 1.1.2.4 Reduplicative compounds
- 1.1.2.5 Phrase-based compounds
- 1.1.2.6 Elative compounds
- 1.1.2.7 Exocentric compounds
- 1.1.2.8 Linking elements
- 1.1.2.9 Separable Complex Verbs and Particle Verbs
- 1.1.2.10 Noun Incorporation Verbs
- 1.1.2.11 Gapping
- 1.2 Derivation
- 1.3 Minor patterns of word formation
- 1.1 Compounding
- 2 Inflection
- 1 Word formation
- Morphology
- Syntax
- Adjectives and adjective phrases (APs)
- 0 Introduction to the AP
- 1 Characteristics and classification of APs
- 2 Complementation of APs
- 3 Modification and degree quantification of APs
- 4 Comparison by comparative, superlative and equative
- 5 Attribution of APs
- 6 Predication of APs
- 7 The partitive adjective construction
- 8 Adverbial use of APs
- 9 Participles and infinitives as APs
- Nouns and Noun Phrases (NPs)
- 0 Introduction to the NP
- 1 Characteristics and Classification of NPs
- 2 Complementation of NPs
- 3 Modification of NPs
- 3.1 Modification of NP by Determiners and APs
- 3.2 Modification of NP by PP
- 3.3 Modification of NP by adverbial clauses
- 3.4 Modification of NP by possessors
- 3.5 Modification of NP by relative clauses
- 3.6 Modification of NP in a cleft construction
- 3.7 Free relative clauses and selected interrogative clauses
- 4 Partitive noun constructions and constructions related to them
- 4.1 The referential partitive construction
- 4.2 The partitive construction of abstract quantity
- 4.3 The numerical partitive construction
- 4.4 The partitive interrogative construction
- 4.5 Adjectival, nominal and nominalised partitive quantifiers
- 4.6 Kind partitives
- 4.7 Partitive predication with a preposition
- 4.8 Bare nominal attribution
- 5 Articles and names
- 6 Pronouns
- 7 Quantifiers, determiners and predeterminers
- 8 Interrogative pronouns
- 9 R-pronouns and the indefinite expletive
- 10 Syntactic functions of Noun Phrases
- Adpositions and Adpositional Phrases (PPs)
- 0 Introduction to the PP
- 1 Characteristics and classification of PPs
- 2 Complementation of PPs
- 3 Modification of PPs
- 4 Bare (intransitive) adpositions
- 5 Predication of PPs
- 6 Form and distribution of adpositions with respect to staticity and construction type
- 7 Adpositional complements and adverbials
- Verbs and Verb Phrases (VPs)
- 0 Introduction to the VP in Saterland Frisian
- 1 Characteristics and classification of verbs
- 2 Unergative and unaccusative subjects and the auxiliary of the perfect
- 3 Evidentiality in relation to perception and epistemicity
- 4 Types of to-infinitival constituents
- 5 Predication
- 5.1 The auxiliary of being and its selection restrictions
- 5.2 The auxiliary of going and its selection restrictions
- 5.3 The auxiliary of continuation and its selection restrictions
- 5.4 The auxiliary of coming and its selection restrictions
- 5.5 Modal auxiliaries and their selection restrictions
- 5.6 Auxiliaries of body posture and aspect and their selection restrictions
- 5.7 Transitive verbs of predication
- 5.8 The auxiliary of doing used as a semantically empty finite auxiliary
- 5.9 Supplementive predication
- 6 The verbal paradigm, irregularity and suppletion
- 7 Verb Second and the word order in main and embedded clauses
- 8 Various aspects of clause structure
- Adjectives and adjective phrases (APs)
-
- General
- Phonology
- Afrikaans phonology
- Segment inventory
- Overview of Afrikaans vowels
- The diphthongised long vowels /e/, /ø/ and /o/
- The unrounded mid-front vowel /ɛ/
- The unrounded low-central vowel /ɑ/
- The unrounded low-central vowel /a/
- The rounded mid-high back vowel /ɔ/
- The rounded high back vowel /u/
- The rounded and unrounded high front vowels /i/ and /y/
- The unrounded and rounded central vowels /ə/ and /œ/
- The diphthongs /əi/, /œy/ and /œu/
- Overview of Afrikaans consonants
- The bilabial plosives /p/ and /b/
- The alveolar plosives /t/ and /d/
- The velar plosives /k/ and /g/
- The bilabial nasal /m/
- The alveolar nasal /n/
- The velar nasal /ŋ/
- The trill /r/
- The lateral liquid /l/
- The alveolar fricative /s/
- The velar fricative /x/
- The labiodental fricatives /f/ and /v/
- The approximants /ɦ/, /j/ and /ʋ/
- Overview of Afrikaans vowels
- Word stress
- The phonetic properties of stress
- Primary stress on monomorphemic words in Afrikaans
- Background to primary stress in monomorphemes in Afrikaans
- Overview of the Main Stress Rule of Afrikaans
- The short vowels of Afrikaans
- Long vowels in monomorphemes
- Primary stress on diphthongs in monomorphemes
- Exceptions
- Stress shifts in place names
- Stress shift towards word-final position
- Stress pattern of reduplications
- Phonological processes
- Vowel related processes
- Consonant related processes
- Homorganic glide insertion
- Phonology-morphology interface
- Phonotactics
- Morphology
- Syntax
- Afrikaans syntax
- Nouns and noun phrases
- Characteristics of the NP
- Classification of nouns
- Complementation of NPs
- Modification of NPs
- Binominal and partitive constructions
- Referential partitive constructions
- Partitive measure nouns
- Numeral partitive constructions
- Partitive question constructions
- Partitive constructions with nominalised quantifiers
- Partitive predication with prepositions
- Binominal name constructions
- Binominal genitive constructions
- Bare nominal attribution
- Articles and names
- Pronouns
- Quantifiers, determiners and predeterminers
- Syntactic uses of the noun phrase
- Adjectives and adjective phrases
- Characteristics and classification of the AP
- Complementation of APs
- Modification and Degree Quantification of APs
- Comparison by comparative, superlative and equative degree
- Attribution of APs
- Predication of APs
- The partitive adjective construction
- Adverbial use of APs
- Participles and infinitives as adjectives
- Verbs and verb phrases
- Characterisation and classification
- Argument structure
- Verb frame alternations
- Complements of non-main verbs
- Verb clusters
- Complement clauses
- Adverbial modification
- Word order in the clause: Introduction
- Word order in the clause: position of the finite Verb
- Word order in the clause: Clause-initial position
- Word order in the clause: Extraposition and right-dislocation in the postverbal field
- Word order in the middle field
- Emphatic constructions
- Adpositions and adposition phrases
Clitics are the weak forms of function words (such as pronouns, determiners, auxiliaries, particles, conjunctions, prepositions). Whereas the function and meaning of their weak form is identical to their full/ strong form, clitics are not autonomous words and phonologically resemble affixes. This entails that they are phonologically dependent on a host word to which they attach and with which they form a prosodic constituent. Moreover, they cannot carry main emphasis, accent or focus. What makes clitics special is the fact that they can attach to a wider range of word classes, which makes them (morphologically) different from regular affixes. Furthermore, the syntax of clitics can differ from the syntax of the corresponding strong/ full forms (see section about non-isomorphism below). Introductions to clitics and general discussions of the status of clitics can be found in Booij (1995), Wackernagel (1892), Zwicky (1977), Klavans (1982,1985), Spencer and Luís (2012) and Anderson (2005).
The examples in tables (1)-(4) show which word sorts can form clitics.
Weak form | Strong form | |
'k /k/or ik /ɪk=/, /ək/ | ik /ɪk/ | 1SG.SBJ |
me /mə/ | mij /mɛi/ | 1SG.OBJ |
je /jə/ | jij /jɛi/ | 2SG.SBJ |
je /jə/ | jou /jɑu/ | 2SG.OBJ |
ie /i/ | hij /hɛi/ | 3SG.SBJ.M |
'm /əm/ | hem /hɛm/ | 3SG.OBJ.M |
ze /zə/ | zij /zɛi/ | 3SG.SBJ.F |
'r /ər/, d'r /dər/ | haar /har/ | 3SG.OBJ.F |
't /ət/, /t/ | het /hɛt/ | 3SG.N |
we /ʋə/ | wij /ʋɛi/ | 1PL.SBJ |
ons /ɔns/ | 1PL.OBJ | |
jullie /jʏli/ | 2PL.SBJ/OBJ | |
ze /zə/ | zij /zɛi/ | 3PL.SBJ |
ze /zə/ | hen /hɛn/and hun /hʏn/ | 3PL.OBJ |
Weak form | Strong form | |
m'n /mən/ | mijn /mɛin/ | my |
je /jə/ | jouw /jɑu/ | your |
z'n /zən/ | zijn /zɛin/ | his |
'r /ər/, d'r /dər/ | haar /har/ | her |
Weak form | Strong form | |
er /ər/ | /ɛr/ | there |
daar /dər/ | /dar/ | there |
eens /əs/ | /ens/ | once |
Weak form | Strong form | |
't /ət/ | het /hɛt/ | DEF.ART.SG.N |
The full forms of the adverbial pronouns can be used in stressed and unstressed positions.
er is both the allomorph of het which is used in prepositional phrases, always occuring before the preposition and the adverbial pronoun for there. It has the full form /ɛr/ and the weak form /ər/. In contrast to the personal pronouns, the pronouns het and er, cannot be used in stressed positions but instead have to be substituted by their corresponding demonstrative pronoun forms dat /dɑt/ and daar [dar] (with its weak form /dər/ also functioning as a stylistic variant of er).
Since many of the weak forms listed above cannot be derived from their strong counterparts by simple phonological processes of reduction, they are presumed to be listed in the lexicon as such (indicated above by denoting both forms as phonologically stored). This is supported by the fact that in some idiomatic expressions only the weak form is possible:
Most clitics, except ie /i/ and ik /ɪk/, either have schwa as their only vowel or are totally vowelless. They do not form prosodic words of their own since these would require at least one full vowel. Since schwa never bears stress (cf. topics about the mid-central vowel /ə/ and schwa epenthesis) and it is the prosodic word that is the domain of word-stress rules, clitics cannot carry stress either.
It is debated whether clitics have to be treated as a special prosodic catergory. According to Nespor and Vogel (1986) and Hayes (1989) amongst others, the 'clitic group' occupies its own tier between the phonological phrase and the prosodic word (ω) in the prosodic hierarchy. Booij (1995;1996) argues against treating clitics as a separate group but rather for seeing it as a matter of prosodic integration into an adjacent prosodic word.
Another phonological process that applies to a host+clitic-combination after schwa is Prevocalic Schwa Deletion, which states that schwa is deleted if it occurs before another vowel:
Another possibility to resolve hiatus is /n/-insertion, as shown in (3a) and (3b) (cf. also the section about the non-isomorphism between syntactic and prosodic structure below).
Since the domain of this phonological process is the prosodic word, it does not take place between a clitic and a following word. Instead, like in unstressed prefixes, a glottal stop is inserted:
je eet (lekker) | /jə et/ | [jəʔet] | [*jənet] | you eat (nicely) |
Another process that also applies to clitics in Dutch is Homorganic Glide Insertion.
Clitics attaching to the right edge of a word are called enclitics; clitics attaching to the left edge of a word are called proclitics.
For more information concerning this structure and other possibilities of representing encliticization see Booij (1996: 12f.)
For more information about the representation in figure (1) and the discussion about F-level skipping and thus violating the Strict Layer Hypothesis see Booij (1996).
Thus, Dutch proclitics are adjoined to the following prosodic word while Dutch enclitics are incorporated into the preceding prosodic word. Prosodically, proclitics are identical to prefixes while enclitics are identical to suffixes, being supported by evidence from morphologization that diachronically proclitics change to prefixes and enclitics to suffixes (Hopper and Traugott 2003). That there is a difference between pro- and encliticization is also shown by the fact that phonological processes apply to a different degree within the two groups. While, as shown above, enclitics undergo processes such as prevocalic schwa deletion, homorganic glide insertion, /n/-insertion etc., these are not obligatory in proclitics. Here prevocalic schwa deletion only occurs in very casual speech
According to Booij (1995, 1996), in Dutch there is no absolute parameter for the direction of cliticization as proposed by Klavans (1985: 98) but 'a preferred direction of attachment' (ibid) which is encliticization in Dutch. Booij (1996) gives a classification of clitics in Dutch:
- words that encliticize,
- prosodically deficient words that cliticize in the direction in which a host is available, i.e. they preferably encliticize (especially clitics beginning with schwa), but procliticization is possible,
- non-prosodically deficient function words with an optional encliticization.
Clitics can thus function both as proclitics and enclitics in Dutch, except group 1 comprising ie he, its allomorph die and pronominal der her which require a host on its left.
Function words (group 3) do not have to cliticize at all since they can form prosodic words of their own.
The preference for encliticization in Dutch is due to the fact that the language provides left-headed feet (aka trochees), which makes the prosodic incorporation of an (unstressed) clitic easier at the right edge of a word. Consequently, since Dutch generally disprefers prosodic words beginning with a schwa like 't [ət] it(Booij 1995), schwa-initial clitics will preferably be encliticized and resyllabified obligatorily ((Jan) ziet 't ((zi)σ (tət)σ)ω (Jan) sees it).
The determiner de /də/ the shows both directions of cliticization, as the phonological proces of voice assimilation in clitics proves: In (7a) word-internal voicing is shown, while in (7ai) regressive voice assimilation is exhibited.
While pronominal clitics (see table (1) above) can exhibit both directions of cliticization, example (8a) shows that consonantal, i.e. vowelless clitics, such as 'k I or 't it, preferably are subject to encliticization, as claimed by amongst others Booij (1995) and Gussenhoven (1985). That consonantal clitics are indeed left-headed, is indicated by their use in combination with words ending in a consonant where the clitic variant with schwa must be used: Ik pak 't [ɪk pɑk ət]/ [*ɪk pɑkt] I fetch it.
As the example above shows, these vowelless clitics can create consonant clusters which do not form well-formed onsets, like /kb-/'k ben I am or /tf-/'t valt it falls. All these co-occurence restrictions between a clitic and the preceding phonological material suggests that they indeed form a unit together.
Sentence-initially, proclitics are used exceptionlessly, while the same holds true for enclitics in sentence-final position.
The prosodic and syntactic structure of clitics is not isomorphic. An example of this fact is found in (10) where the determiner 't the syntactically belongs to the following noun boek book. Prosodically however, since this clitic needs a host, it gets incorporated into the preceding verb kocht bought, together with which it subsequently forms a prosodic word.
Jan kocht 't boek. |
Syntactic structure: |
[(NP)Jan] [(V)kocht] [(NP)'t boek] |
Prosodic structure: | ||||||||||||||
[jɑn](ω) [[kɔx](σ)[tət](σ)](ω) [buk](ω) | ||||||||||||||
Jan bought the book. |
Evidence for incorporation is found in resyllabification where a blocking of other phonological processes such as optional /n/-deletion after schwa in (11) can be observed:
Zij kochten 't boek. | ||||||||||||||
[zei](ω) [[kɔx](σ)[tə](σ)[nət](σ)](ω) [buk](ω) | ||||||||||||||
They bought the book. |
- 2005Aspects of the theory of cliticsNew YorkOxford University Press
- 1995The phonology of DutchOxfordClarendon Press
- 1995The phonology of DutchOxfordClarendon Press
- 1995The phonology of DutchOxfordClarendon Press
- 1995The phonology of DutchOxfordClarendon Press
- 1995The phonology of DutchOxfordClarendon Press
- 1996Cliticization as prosodic integration: the case of DutchThe Linguistic Review13219-242
- 1996Cliticization as prosodic integration: the case of DutchThe Linguistic Review13219-242
- 1996Cliticization as prosodic integration: the case of DutchThe Linguistic Review13219-242
- 1996Cliticization as prosodic integration: the case of DutchThe Linguistic Review13219-242
- 1996Cliticization as prosodic integration: the case of DutchThe Linguistic Review13219-242
- 1996Cliticization as prosodic integration: the case of DutchThe Linguistic Review13219-242
- 1985Over de fonologie van Nederlandse cliticaSpektator15180-200
- 1989The prosodic hierarchy in meterKiparsky, Paul & Youmans, Gilbert (eds.)Phonetics and phonology1: Rhythm and meterSan DiegoAcademic Press201-260
- 2003GrammaticalizationCambridge U.K.Cambridge University Press
- 1982Some problems in a theory of cliticsUniversity of MichiganThesis
- 1985The independence of syntax and phonology in cliticizationLanguage6195-120
- 1985The independence of syntax and phonology in cliticizationLanguage6195-120
- 1986Prosodic phonologyStudies in generative grammar 28DordrechtForis Publ.
- 2012Clitics: an introductionCambridge, UKCambridge University Press
- 1892Über ein Gesetz der indogermanischen WortstellungIndogermanische Forschungen1333-436
- 1977On CliticsBloomington