- Dutch
- Frisian
- Saterfrisian
- Afrikaans
-
- Phonology
- Segment inventory
- Phonotactics
- Phonological processes
- Phonology-morphology interface
- Word stress
- Primary stress in simplex words
- Monomorphemic words
- Diachronic aspects
- Generalizations on stress placement
- Default penultimate stress
- Lexical stress
- The closed penult restriction
- Final closed syllables
- The diphthong restriction
- Superheavy syllables (SHS)
- The three-syllable window
- Segmental restrictions
- Phonetic correlates
- Stress shifts in loanwords
- Quantity-sensitivity
- Secondary stress
- Vowel reduction in unstressed syllables
- Stress in complex words
- Primary stress in simplex words
- Accent & intonation
- Clitics
- Spelling
- Morphology
- Word formation
- Compounding
- Nominal compounds
- Verbal compounds
- Adjectival compounds
- Affixoids
- Coordinative compounds
- Synthetic compounds
- Reduplicative compounds
- Phrase-based compounds
- Elative compounds
- Exocentric compounds
- Linking elements
- Separable complex verbs (SCVs)
- Gapping of complex words
- Particle verbs
- Copulative compounds
- Derivation
- Numerals
- Derivation: inputs and input restrictions
- The meaning of affixes
- Non-native morphology
- Cohering and non-cohering affixes
- Prefixation
- Suffixation
- Nominal suffixation: person nouns
- Conversion
- Pseudo-participles
- Bound forms
- Nouns
- Nominal prefixes
- Nominal suffixes
- -aal and -eel
- -aar
- -aard
- -aat
- -air
- -aris
- -ast
- Diminutives
- -dom
- -een
- -ees
- -el (nominal)
- -elaar
- -enis
- -er (nominal)
- -erd
- -erik
- -es
- -eur
- -euse
- ge...te
- -heid
- -iaan, -aan
- -ief
- -iek
- -ier
- -ier (French)
- -ière
- -iet
- -igheid
- -ij and allomorphs
- -ijn
- -in
- -ing
- -isme
- -ist
- -iteit
- -ling
- -oir
- -oot
- -rice
- -schap
- -schap (de)
- -schap (het)
- -sel
- -st
- -ster
- -t
- -tal
- -te
- -voud
- Verbs
- Adjectives
- Adverbs
- Univerbation
- Neo-classical word formation
- Construction-dependent morphology
- Morphological productivity
- Compounding
- Inflection
- Inflection and derivation
- Allomorphy
- The interface between phonology and morphology
- Word formation
- Syntax
- Preface and acknowledgements
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
- 1 Characterization and classification
- 2 Projection of verb phrases I:Argument structure
- 3 Projection of verb phrases II:Verb frame alternations
- Introduction
- 3.1. Main types
- 3.2. Alternations involving the external argument
- 3.3. Alternations of noun phrases and PPs
- 3.3.1. Dative/PP alternations (dative shift)
- 3.3.1.1. Dative alternation with aan-phrases (recipients)
- 3.3.1.2. Dative alternation with naar-phrases (goals)
- 3.3.1.3. Dative alternation with van-phrases (sources)
- 3.3.1.4. Dative alternation with bij-phrases (possessors)
- 3.3.1.5. Dative alternation with voor-phrases (benefactives)
- 3.3.1.6. Conclusion
- 3.3.1.7. Bibliographical notes
- 3.3.2. Accusative/PP alternations
- 3.3.3. Nominative/PP alternations
- 3.3.1. Dative/PP alternations (dative shift)
- 3.4. Some apparent cases of verb frame alternation
- 3.5. Bibliographical notes
- 4 Projection of verb phrases IIIa:Selection of clauses/verb phrases
- 5 Projection of verb phrases IIIb:Argument and complementive clauses
- Introduction
- 5.1. Finite argument clauses
- 5.2. Infinitival argument clauses
- 5.3. Complementive clauses
- 6 Projection of verb phrases IIIc:Complements of non-main verbs
- 7 Projection of verb phrases IIId:Verb clusters
- 8 Projection of verb phrases IV: Adverbial modification
- 9 Word order in the clause I:General introduction
- 10 Word order in the clause II:Position of the finite verb (verb-first/second)
- 11 Word order in the clause III:Clause-initial position (wh-movement)
- Introduction
- 11.1. The formation of V1- and V2-clauses
- 11.2. Clause-initial position remains (phonetically) empty
- 11.3. Clause-initial position is filled
- 12 Word order in the clause IV:Postverbal field (extraposition)
- 13 Word order in the clause V: Middle field (scrambling)
- 14 Main-clause external elements
- Nouns and Noun Phrases
- 1 Characterization and classification
- 2 Projection of noun phrases I: complementation
- Introduction
- 2.1. General observations
- 2.2. Prepositional and nominal complements
- 2.3. Clausal complements
- 2.4. Bibliographical notes
- 3 Projection of noun phrases II: modification
- Introduction
- 3.1. Restrictive and non-restrictive modifiers
- 3.2. Premodification
- 3.3. Postmodification
- 3.3.1. Adpositional phrases
- 3.3.2. Relative clauses
- 3.3.3. Infinitival clauses
- 3.3.4. A special case: clauses referring to a proposition
- 3.3.5. Adjectival phrases
- 3.3.6. Adverbial postmodification
- 3.4. Bibliographical notes
- 4 Projection of noun phrases III: binominal constructions
- Introduction
- 4.1. Binominal constructions without a preposition
- 4.2. Binominal constructions with a preposition
- 4.3. Bibliographical notes
- 5 Determiners: articles and pronouns
- Introduction
- 5.1. Articles
- 5.2. Pronouns
- 5.3. Bibliographical notes
- 6 Numerals and quantifiers
- 7 Pre-determiners
- Introduction
- 7.1. The universal quantifier al 'all' and its alternants
- 7.2. The pre-determiner heel 'all/whole'
- 7.3. A note on focus particles
- 7.4. Bibliographical notes
- 8 Syntactic uses of noun phrases
- Adjectives and Adjective Phrases
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- 2 Projection of adjective phrases I: Complementation
- 3 Projection of adjective phrases II: Modification
- 4 Projection of adjective phrases III: Comparison
- 5 Attributive use of the adjective phrase
- 6 Predicative use of the adjective phrase
- 7 The partitive genitive construction
- 8 Adverbial use of the adjective phrase
- 9 Participles and infinitives: their adjectival use
- 10 Special constructions
- Adpositions and adpositional phrases
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- Introduction
- 1.1. Characterization of the category adposition
- 1.2. A formal classification of adpositional phrases
- 1.3. A semantic classification of adpositional phrases
- 1.3.1. Spatial adpositions
- 1.3.2. Temporal adpositions
- 1.3.3. Non-spatial/temporal prepositions
- 1.4. Borderline cases
- 1.5. Bibliographical notes
- 2 Projection of adpositional phrases: Complementation
- 3 Projection of adpositional phrases: Modification
- 4 Syntactic uses of the adpositional phrase
- 5 R-pronominalization and R-words
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- Phonology
-
- General
- Phonology
- Segment inventory
- Phonotactics
- Phonological Processes
- Assimilation
- Vowel nasalization
- Syllabic sonorants
- Final devoicing
- Fake geminates
- Vowel hiatus resolution
- Vowel reduction introduction
- Schwa deletion
- Schwa insertion
- /r/-deletion
- d-insertion
- {s/z}-insertion
- t-deletion
- Intrusive stop formation
- Breaking
- Vowel shortening
- h-deletion
- Replacement of the glide w
- Word stress
- Clitics
- Allomorphy
- Orthography of Frisian
- Morphology
- Inflection
- Word formation
- Derivation
- Prefixation
- Infixation
- Suffixation
- Nominal suffixes
- Verbal suffixes
- Adjectival suffixes
- Adverbial suffixes
- Numeral suffixes
- Interjectional suffixes
- Onomastic suffixes
- Conversion
- Compositions
- Derivation
- Syntax
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Unergative and unaccusative subjects
- Evidentiality
- To-infinitival clauses
- Predication and noun incorporation
- Ellipsis
- Imperativus-pro-Infinitivo
- Expression of irrealis
- Embedded Verb Second
- Agreement
- Negation
- Nouns & Noun Phrases
- Classification
- Complementation
- Modification
- Partitive noun constructions
- Referential partitive constructions
- Partitive measure nouns
- Numeral partitive constructions
- Partitive question constructions
- Nominalised quantifiers
- Kind partitives
- Partitive predication with prepositions
- Bare nominal attributions
- Articles and names
- Pronouns
- Quantifiers and (pre)determiners
- Interrogative pronouns
- R-pronouns
- Syntactic uses
- Adjective Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Complementation
- Modification and degree quantification
- Comparison by degree
- Comparative
- Superlative
- Equative
- Attribution
- Agreement
- Attributive adjectives vs. prenominal elements
- Complex adjectives
- Noun ellipsis
- Co-occurring adjectives
- Predication
- Partitive adjective constructions
- Adverbial use
- Participles and infinitives
- Adposition Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Complementation
- Modification
- Intransitive adpositions
- Predication
- Preposition stranding
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
-
- General
- Morphology
- Morphology
- 1 Word formation
- 1.1 Compounding
- 1.1.1 Compounds and their heads
- 1.1.2 Special types of compounds
- 1.1.2.1 Affixoids
- 1.1.2.2 Coordinative compounds
- 1.1.2.3 Synthetic compounds and complex pseudo-participles
- 1.1.2.4 Reduplicative compounds
- 1.1.2.5 Phrase-based compounds
- 1.1.2.6 Elative compounds
- 1.1.2.7 Exocentric compounds
- 1.1.2.8 Linking elements
- 1.1.2.9 Separable Complex Verbs and Particle Verbs
- 1.1.2.10 Noun Incorporation Verbs
- 1.1.2.11 Gapping
- 1.2 Derivation
- 1.3 Minor patterns of word formation
- 1.1 Compounding
- 2 Inflection
- 1 Word formation
- Morphology
- Syntax
- Adjectives and adjective phrases (APs)
- 0 Introduction to the AP
- 1 Characteristics and classification of APs
- 2 Complementation of APs
- 3 Modification and degree quantification of APs
- 4 Comparison by comparative, superlative and equative
- 5 Attribution of APs
- 6 Predication of APs
- 7 The partitive adjective construction
- 8 Adverbial use of APs
- 9 Participles and infinitives as APs
- Nouns and Noun Phrases (NPs)
- 0 Introduction to the NP
- 1 Characteristics and Classification of NPs
- 2 Complementation of NPs
- 3 Modification of NPs
- 3.1 Modification of NP by Determiners and APs
- 3.2 Modification of NP by PP
- 3.3 Modification of NP by adverbial clauses
- 3.4 Modification of NP by possessors
- 3.5 Modification of NP by relative clauses
- 3.6 Modification of NP in a cleft construction
- 3.7 Free relative clauses and selected interrogative clauses
- 4 Partitive noun constructions and constructions related to them
- 4.1 The referential partitive construction
- 4.2 The partitive construction of abstract quantity
- 4.3 The numerical partitive construction
- 4.4 The partitive interrogative construction
- 4.5 Adjectival, nominal and nominalised partitive quantifiers
- 4.6 Kind partitives
- 4.7 Partitive predication with a preposition
- 4.8 Bare nominal attribution
- 5 Articles and names
- 6 Pronouns
- 7 Quantifiers, determiners and predeterminers
- 8 Interrogative pronouns
- 9 R-pronouns and the indefinite expletive
- 10 Syntactic functions of Noun Phrases
- Adpositions and Adpositional Phrases (PPs)
- 0 Introduction to the PP
- 1 Characteristics and classification of PPs
- 2 Complementation of PPs
- 3 Modification of PPs
- 4 Bare (intransitive) adpositions
- 5 Predication of PPs
- 6 Form and distribution of adpositions with respect to staticity and construction type
- 7 Adpositional complements and adverbials
- Verbs and Verb Phrases (VPs)
- 0 Introduction to the VP in Saterland Frisian
- 1 Characteristics and classification of verbs
- 2 Unergative and unaccusative subjects and the auxiliary of the perfect
- 3 Evidentiality in relation to perception and epistemicity
- 4 Types of to-infinitival constituents
- 5 Predication
- 5.1 The auxiliary of being and its selection restrictions
- 5.2 The auxiliary of going and its selection restrictions
- 5.3 The auxiliary of continuation and its selection restrictions
- 5.4 The auxiliary of coming and its selection restrictions
- 5.5 Modal auxiliaries and their selection restrictions
- 5.6 Auxiliaries of body posture and aspect and their selection restrictions
- 5.7 Transitive verbs of predication
- 5.8 The auxiliary of doing used as a semantically empty finite auxiliary
- 5.9 Supplementive predication
- 6 The verbal paradigm, irregularity and suppletion
- 7 Verb Second and the word order in main and embedded clauses
- 8 Various aspects of clause structure
- Adjectives and adjective phrases (APs)
-
- General
- Phonology
- Afrikaans phonology
- Segment inventory
- Overview of Afrikaans vowels
- The diphthongised long vowels /e/, /ø/ and /o/
- The unrounded mid-front vowel /ɛ/
- The unrounded low-central vowel /ɑ/
- The unrounded low-central vowel /a/
- The rounded mid-high back vowel /ɔ/
- The rounded high back vowel /u/
- The rounded and unrounded high front vowels /i/ and /y/
- The unrounded and rounded central vowels /ə/ and /œ/
- The diphthongs /əi/, /œy/ and /œu/
- Overview of Afrikaans consonants
- The bilabial plosives /p/ and /b/
- The alveolar plosives /t/ and /d/
- The velar plosives /k/ and /g/
- The bilabial nasal /m/
- The alveolar nasal /n/
- The velar nasal /ŋ/
- The trill /r/
- The lateral liquid /l/
- The alveolar fricative /s/
- The velar fricative /x/
- The labiodental fricatives /f/ and /v/
- The approximants /ɦ/, /j/ and /ʋ/
- Overview of Afrikaans vowels
- Word stress
- The phonetic properties of stress
- Primary stress on monomorphemic words in Afrikaans
- Background to primary stress in monomorphemes in Afrikaans
- Overview of the Main Stress Rule of Afrikaans
- The short vowels of Afrikaans
- Long vowels in monomorphemes
- Primary stress on diphthongs in monomorphemes
- Exceptions
- Stress shifts in place names
- Stress shift towards word-final position
- Stress pattern of reduplications
- Phonological processes
- Vowel related processes
- Consonant related processes
- Homorganic glide insertion
- Phonology-morphology interface
- Phonotactics
- Morphology
- Syntax
- Afrikaans syntax
- Nouns and noun phrases
- Characteristics of the NP
- Classification of nouns
- Complementation of NPs
- Modification of NPs
- Binominal and partitive constructions
- Referential partitive constructions
- Partitive measure nouns
- Numeral partitive constructions
- Partitive question constructions
- Partitive constructions with nominalised quantifiers
- Partitive predication with prepositions
- Binominal name constructions
- Binominal genitive constructions
- Bare nominal attribution
- Articles and names
- Pronouns
- Quantifiers, determiners and predeterminers
- Syntactic uses of the noun phrase
- Adjectives and adjective phrases
- Characteristics and classification of the AP
- Complementation of APs
- Modification and Degree Quantification of APs
- Comparison by comparative, superlative and equative degree
- Attribution of APs
- Predication of APs
- The partitive adjective construction
- Adverbial use of APs
- Participles and infinitives as adjectives
- Verbs and verb phrases
- Characterisation and classification
- Argument structure
- Verb frame alternations
- Complements of non-main verbs
- Verb clusters
- Complement clauses
- Adverbial modification
- Word order in the clause: Introduction
- Word order in the clause: position of the finite Verb
- Word order in the clause: Clause-initial position
- Word order in the clause: Extraposition and right-dislocation in the postverbal field
- Word order in the middle field
- Emphatic constructions
- Adpositions and adposition phrases
The previous sections have only discussed examples with isolated adjectives for reasons of simplicity, but, of course, these examples in fact involve full APs, which incidentally do not contain any complements or adjuncts. This section will have a closer look at the attributive use of more complex APs that do contain a complement or an adjunct.
This subsection discusses the attributive use of APs containing a complement. We start in Subsection A by discussing APs with nominal complements, which is followed in Subsection B by a discussion of APs with PP-complements.
Section 2.2 has shown that a limited set of adjectives take a nominal complement, which (at least in German) may appear either with genitive or with dative case. For convenience, we repeat the enumeration of these adjectives in (76). This subsection considers the question of what happens if these adjectives are used attributively.
a. | Genitive: bewust'conscious', deelachtig'partaking', gewend'used', indachtig'mindful', moe/zat/beu'weary', machtig'in command of' |
b. | Dative: aangeboren'innate', beschoren'given', bespaard'spared', duidelijk/helder'clear', goedgezind'well disposed', (on)bekend'known', toegewijd/toegedaan'devoted', vertrouwd'familiar', vreemd'foreign'Dative: aangeboren'innate', beschoren'given', bespaard'spared', duidelijk/helder'clear', goedgezind'well disposed', (on)bekend'known', toegewijd/toegedaan'devoted', vertrouwd'familiar', vreemd'foreign' |
If adjectives with a dative complement are used attributively, the head noun must correspond to the subject of the corresponding copular construction, while the dative shows up as a complement of the adjective. This is illustrated in the examples in (77); example (77b') perhaps becomes slightly marked if the pronoun is replaced by a non-pronominal phrase like Jan or de directeur'the manager'.
a. | Het taalvermogen | is (de mens) | aangeboren. | |
the linguistic.competence | is the man | innate | ||
'Linguistic competence is innate (to man).' |
a'. | het | (de mens) | aangeboren | taalvermogen | |
the | the man | innate | linguistic.competence | ||
'linguistic competence, innate to man' |
b. | Dit probleem | is (hem/de directeur) | bekend. | |
this problem | is him/the manager | known |
b'. | het | (hem/?de directeur) | bekende | probleem | |
the | him/the manager | known | problem |
The examples in (77) also show that if the dative object is optional in the copular construction, it is also optional in the attributive construction. The examples in (78) show that if the dative object is obligatory in the copular construction, it must also appear in the attributive construction. Note that. like (77b'), (78b') becomes marked if the pronoun is replaced by a non-pronominal phrase like Jan or de directeur'the manager'.
a. | Dat lot | was *(hem) | beschoren. | |
that destiny | was him | given |
a'. | het | *(hem) | beschoren | lot | |
the | him | given | destiny |
b. | Dat lot | bleef | *(hem) | niet | bespaard. | |
that destiny | remained | him | not | spared | ||
'He has been spared that destiny.' |
b'. | het | *(hem) | bespaarde | lot | |
the | him | spared | destiny |
If the dative noun phrase alternates with an aan-PP, this is also possible if the AP is used attributively. Although judgments are subtle, it seems that the dative phrase if the noun phrase is pronominal, and that the PP is preferred if it is non-pronominal; the judgments given are ours. The doubly-primed examples show that, just like the dative noun phrases, the aan-PPs must precede the modified noun.
a. | de | hem/?Peter | gehoorzame | hond | |
the | him/Peter | obedient | dog |
a'. | de | aan Peter/(?)hem | gehoorzame | hond | |
the | to Peter/him | obedient | dog |
a''. | * | de | gehoorzame | hond aan Peter/hem |
b. | de | hem/?Peter | trouwe | hond | |
the | him/Peter | loyal | dog |
b'. | de | aan Peter/(?)hem | trouwe | hond | |
the | to Peter/him | loyal | dog |
b''. | * | de trouwe hond aan Peter/hem |
The examples in (80) provide cases in which the dative phrase is licensed by the evaluative degree element te'too'. The primed examples show that the dative noun phrase (given in the primeless examples) alternates with a voor-PP, which again seems to be preferred if the dative phrase is non-pronominal. The doubly-primed examples show that, just like the dative noun phrase, the voor-PP must precede the noun.
a. | een | (mij/?Peter) | te moeilijk | boek | |
a | me/Peter | too difficult | book | ||
'a book too difficult for me/Peter' |
a'. | een | voor Peter/mij | te moeilijk boek | |
a | for Peter/me | too difficult book |
a''. | ?? | een te moeilijk boek voor Peter/mij |
b. | een | (mij/?Peter) | te koud | zwembad | |
a | me/Peter | too cold | swimming.pool | ||
'a swimming pool too cold for me' |
b'. | een | voor Peter/mij | te koud | zwembad | |
a | for Peter/me | too cold | swimming.pool |
b''. | ?? | een te koud | zwembad voor Peter/mij |
The examples in (81) show that adjectives cannot readily be used attributively if they take a genitive argument (although German has been reported to be more permissive in this respect). The marked status of these examples may be related to the observation in the previous subsection that dative phrases also seem less acceptable if they are non-pronominal: given that genitive phrases typically express new information, they normally cannot be pronominalized.
a. | ? | de | (zich) | het probleem | bewuste | jongen |
the | refl | the problem | conscious | boy |
b. | ?? | de | het geluk | deelachtige | jongen |
the | the happiness | partaking | boy |
c. | ?? | het | de doden | indachtige | meisje |
the | the dead | mindful | girl |
d. | ?? | het | de opera | moeie/zatte/beue | meisje |
the | the opera | weary | girl |
e. | ? | het | de Franse taal | machtige | meisje |
the | the French language | in.command.of | girl |
Unlike what we found with the dative phrases, the examples in (81) cannot be saved by realizing the genitive phrase as a van-PP, which is illustrated in (82) for those examples that allow the alternation. For completeness’ sake, note that placing the van-PP in postnominal position has a further deteriorating effect.
a. | de | (zich) | <?van het probleem> | bewuste | jongen <*van het probleem> | |
the | refl | of the problem | conscious | boy |
b. | het | <*?van de opera> | moeie/zatte/beue | meisje <*van de opera> | |
the | of the opera | weary | girl |
Attributive use of fixed expressions like het spoor bijster zijn'to be lost/confused', de stad meester zijn'to be in command of the city' or iets kwijt zijn'to have lost something' is excluded. The only way to express the intended idea is by using the present participle of the copular verb zijn'to be', as shown in (83).
a. | de | het spoor | bijster zijnde/*bijstere | jongen | |
the | the track | lost being/lost | boy | ||
'the boy that has lost his way' |
b. | het | de stad | meester zijnde/*meestere | leger | |
the | the city | in.command.of being/in.command.of | army | ||
'the army that is in command of the city' |
c. | de | zijn sleutels | kwijt zijnde/*kwijte | jongen | |
the | his keys | lost being/lost | boy | ||
'the boy that has lost his keys' |
This subsection discusses the attributive use of APs with a PP-complement, subsection 1 starts with cases in which the preposition is complemented by a regular noun phrase. This is followed in Subsection 2 by a discussion of anticipatory pronominal PPs introducing a complement clause.
The examples in (84) and (85) show that, unlike most adjectives, pseudo-participles like verliefd'in-love' and deverbal adjectives like afhankelijk'dependent' can be either preceded or followed by their PP-complement. The PP-complement is part of the AP, as is clear from the fact that the PP can be pied-piped by topicalization of the AP (the constituency test) and from the fact that the pre-adjectival PP is situated between the modifier erg'very'/volkomen'completely' and the adjective; cf. Sections 2.3.1, sub III, and 4.3.1 for discussion.
a. | De man | is zeker | erg | verliefd | op zijn vrouw. | |
the man | is certainly | very | in.love | with his wife |
a'. | [Erg verliefd op zijn vrouw] is de man zeker. |
b. | De man is zeker erg op zijn vrouw verliefd. |
b'. | [Erg op zijn vrouw verliefd] is de man zeker. |
a. | De student | is niet | volkomen | afhankelijk | van zijn beurs. | |
the student | is not | completely | dependent | on his grant |
a'. | [Volkomen afhankelijk van zijn beurs] is de student niet. |
b. | De student is niet volkomen van zijn beurs afhankelijk. |
b'. | [Volkomen van zijn beurs afhankelijk] is de student niet. |
We conclude from this that in both orders the adjective and the PP are part of a single AP, and, consequently, we expect that the APs in (84) and (85) can be used attributively on both orders. The examples in (86) show, however, that this expectation is not fully borne out: the APs can only be used attributively if the PP precedes the adjective.
a. | * | een erg verliefde op zijn vrouw man |
a'. | een | erg | op | zijn vrouw | verliefde | man | |
a | very | with | his wife | in.love | man | ||
'a man who is very in love with his wife' |
b. | * | een volkomen afhankelijke van zijn beurs student |
b'. | een | volkomen | van zijn beurs | afhankelijke | student | |
a | completely | on his grant | dependent | student | ||
'a student who is entirely dependent on his grant' |
This has led to the conclusion that attributively used adjectives must be immediately adjacent to the nominal projection N# they modify, that is, that the configuration [.. [AP XP .. XP] N#] is excluded if the string XP between the attributive adjective and the head noun is non-null. This constraint is known as the Head-final Filter, since, in effect, it requires that the adjective be the rightmost element in its own projection. Note that we cannot simply say that the adjective must be immediately adjacent to the noun it modifies given that this would incorrectly exclude the stacking of adjectives, as in de mooie, blauwe stoel'the beautiful, blue chair'; see Section 5.5 for discussion.
Head-Final Filter on attributive adjectives: The structure [NP [AP ADJ XP] N#] is unacceptable, if XP is phonetically non-null and N# is a bare head noun or a noun preceded by an adjective phrase: [(AP) N]. |
As can be seen in (88), the adjective gek'fond' must be followed by the PP op zijn vrouw'of his wife'. The constituency test in the primed examples shows that the string gek op zijn vrouw must again be considered one constituent, namely an AP.
a. | De man | is zeker | gek | op zijn vrouw. | |
the man | is certainly | fond | of his wife |
a'. | [Gek op zijn vrouw] is de man zeker. |
b. | * | De man is zeker op zijn vrouw gek. |
b'. | * | [Op zijn vrouw gek] is de man zeker. |
Given the Head-final Filter in (87), we expect that this AP cannot be used attributively: the order gek op zijn vrouw violates (87), and the order op zijn vrouw gek is impossible under any circumstance. That this expectation is borne out is illustrated in (89).
a. | * | een gekke op zijn vrouw man |
b. | * | een op zijn vrouw gekke man |
Adjectives such as trots'proud', tevreden'satisfied' and bang'afraid' give rise to a slightly degraded result in constructions comparable to (88b'); cf. Section 2.3.1, sub IIB. These adjectives also give rise to a marked result in attributive position if they are preceded by their PP-complement; the result is ungrammatical if the PP-complement follows the adjective, which of course follows from the filter in (87). This is illustrated in (90).
a. | de | <(?)op zijn kinderen> | trotse <*op zijn kinderen> | man | |
the | of his children | proud | man |
b. | de | <(?)over het resultaat> | tevreden <*over het resultaat> | jongen | |
the | about the result | satisfied | boy |
c. | het | <(?)voor de hond> | bange <*voor de hond> | meisje | |
the | of the dog | afraid | girl |
Insofar as the examples in (90) are acceptable, this is the result of movement into some AP-external position. A first indication of this is the fact that the PP must precede the intensifier of the adjective, as shown by the examples in (91).
a. | de | <*erg> | op zijn kinderen <(?)erg> | trotse | man | |
the | very | of his children | fond | man |
b. | de | <*erg> | over het resultaat <(?)erg> | tevreden | jongen | |
the | very | about the result | satisfied | boy |
c. | het | <*erg> | voor de hond <(?)erg> | bange | meisje | |
the | very | of the dog | afraid | girl |
The examples in (92) provide more support for the claim that the PPs in (90) are external to the AP by showing that they must also precede AP-external adverbial material.
a. | de | <*altijd> | op zijn kinderen <?altijd> | trotse | man | |
the | always | of his children | fond | man |
b. | de | <*voortdurend> | over het resultaat <?voordurend> | tevreden | jongen | |
the | continuously | about the result | satisfied | boy |
c. | het | <*nog steeds> | voor de hond <?nog steeds> | bange | meisje | |
the | prt still | of the dog | afraid | girl |
Section 2.1, sub II, has discussed that some adjectives may take a clausal prepositional complement. Consider the two primeless examples in (93), which require that the anticipatory pronominal PP-complement of the adjective be present. The primed examples show that these adjectives cannot be used attributively. Of course, the ungrammaticality of (93a') may be due to the Head-final Filter in (87), since the adjective ziek is separated from the head-noun by the stranded preposition van. However, since this filter does not account for the ungrammaticality of (93b'), in which the stranded preposition precedes the attributively used pseudo-participle gekant, the ungrammaticality is apparently due to the presence of the (in this case finite) clausal complement.
a. | Jan is er | ziek | van | dat | jij | steeds | zeurt. | |
Jan is there | fed.up | with | that | you | continually | nag | ||
'Jan is fed up with it that you are nagging all the time.' |
a'. | * | de | er | ziek(e) | van | jongen | dat | jij | steeds | zeurt |
the | there | fed.up | with | boy | that | you | continually | nag |
b. | Jan is er | tegen | gekant | dat | Marie uitgenodigd | wordt. | |
Jan is there | against | opposed | that | Marie invited | is | ||
'Jan is opposed to it that Marie is invited.' |
b'. | * | de | er | tegen | gekante | jongen | dat | Marie uitgenodigd | wordt |
the | there | against | opposed | boy | that | Marie invited | is |
Adjectives such as boos'angry' and tevreden'satisfied' in (94) may also appear without the anticipatory pronominal PP. The fact that attributive use of these adjectives is excluded, regardless of the presence or absence of the pronominal prepositional phrase, shows again that the impossibility of using these adjectives attributively is apparently due to the presence of the clausal complement.
a. | Jan is (er) | boos | (over) | dat | hij | niet | uitgenodigd | is. | |
Jan is there | angry | about | that | he | not | prt.-invited | is | ||
'Jan is angry (about it) that he isnʼt invited.' |
a'. | * | de | (er) | boze | (over) | jongen | dat | hij | niet | uitgenodigd | is |
the | there | angry | about | boy | that | he | not | prt.-invited | is |
b. | Jan is (er) | tevreden | (over) | dat | hij | uitgenodigd | is. | |
Jan is there | satisfied | about | that | he | invited | is | ||
'Jan is satisfied (about it) that heʼs invited.' |
b'. | * | de | (er) | tevreden | (over) | jongen | dat | hij | uitgenodigd | is |
the | there | satisfied | about | boy | that | he | prt.-invited | is |
Although it is tempting to attribute the ungrammaticality of the primed examples in (93) and (94) to the presence of the clausal complement, it should be noted that the examples with the split anticipatory pronominal PP are also ungrammatical if the clauses are dropped; adjectives such as boos and tevreden can only be used in attributive position if the split anticipatory pronominal PP is absent, as in (95a). The fact that (95b), in which the pronominal PP is moved leftward, is acceptable does not bear on the issue at hand, given that anticipatory pronominal PPs invariantly have the form er + P; consequently we are dealing in this example with a pronominalized PP-complement of the form discussed in Subsection 1 above.
a. | de | boze/tevreden | jongen | |
the | angry/satisfied | boy |
b. | de | daarover | zeer boze/tevreden | jongen | |
the | about.that | very angry/satisfied | boy |
There are no special restrictions on the attributive use of APs containing pre-adjectival modifiers like erg in een erg beleefde jongen'a very polite boy'. Modifiers that follow the adjective, on the other hand, do show special behavior if the adjective is used attributively. This will be discussed in this subsection: Subsection A will start with a discussion of equative, comparative, and superlative adjectives followed by an als/dan-phrase and of adjectives modified by zo + degree clause; Subsection B concludes with a discussion of adjectives modified by genoeg'enough' and zo ... mogelijk'as ... as possible'.
We have seen in Subsection I that the Head-final Filter on attributive adjectives in (87) prohibits the placement of lexical material between attributively used adjectives and the head noun. Given this, we would expect that modifiers that obligatorily follow the modified adjective are excluded if the latter is used attributively. However, such examples are sometimes saved by a “repair" strategy that consists in placing the offending element after the noun. This holds especially for als/dan/van-phrases associated with equative/comparative and superlative adjectives, discussed in Chapter 4, and degree modifiers like net zo'(just) as', discussed in Section 3.1.3. This is illustrated in (96); for completeness’ sake, the primed examples demonstrate again that the adjective and the als/dan/van-phrase constitute a single AP.
a. | Jouw begeleider | is zeker | niet | even/net zo | vriendelijk | als de mijne. | |
your supervisor | is certainly | not | as/just as | friendly | as the mine | ||
'Your supervisor is certainly not as friendly as mine.' |
a'. | [Even/net zo vriendelijk als de mijne] is jouw begeleider zeker niet. |
b. | Jouw begeleider | is zeker | niet | vriendelijker | dan de mijne. | |
your supervisor | is certainly | not | friendlier | than the mine | ||
'Your supervisor is certainly not friendlier than mine.' |
b'. | [Vriendelijker dan de mijne] is jouw begeleider zeker niet. |
c. | Die jongen | is beslist | het leukst | van deze groep. | |
that boy | is definitely | the nicest | of this group |
c'. | [Het leukst van deze groep] is die jongen beslist. |
The examples in (97) show that, as predicted by the Head-final Filter, the als/dan/van-phrases cannot be placed in between the adjective and the head noun, but also that these examples can be saved from this filter by placing the als/dan/van-phrase after the modified head noun.
a. | * | een | even/net zo | vriendelijke | als de mijne | begeleider |
a | as/just as | friendly | as the mine | supervisor |
a'. | een | even/net zo | vriendelijke | begeleider | als de mijne | (is een zegen.) | |
a | as/just as | friendly | supervisor | as the mine | is a bless | ||
'A supervisor as friendly as I have, (is a blessing).' |
b. | * | een | vriendelijkere | dan de mijne | begeleider |
a | friendlier | than the mine | supervisor |
b'. | een | vriendelijkere | begeleider | dan de mijne | (bestaat niet.) | |
a | friendlier | supervisor | than the mine | exists not | ||
'A friendlier supervisor than mine doesnʼt exist.' |
c. | * | de | leukste | van deze groep | jongen |
the | nicest | of this group | boy |
c'. | de | leukste jongen | van deze groep | |
the | nicest boy | of this group |
The “repair" strategy does not work for the PP-complement of adjectives: example (98) shows that placement of the PP-complement op zijn vrouw after the modified head noun man does not improve the result. Possibly, the difference between the examples in (97), on the one hand, and the examples in (98), on the other, is related to the fact that in (97) the als/dan-phrase is not a complement of the adjective itself, but depends on the presence of the intensifier even/net zo or the comparative/superlative morpheme, whereas the PP op zijn vrouw is directly selected by the adjectives verliefd and gek.
a. | * | een | verliefde | <op zijn vrouw> | man <op zijn vrouw> |
an | in.love | with his wife | man |
b. | * | een | gekke | <op zijn vrouw> | man <op zijn vrouw> |
a | fond | of his wife | man |
Possibly, the difference between the examples in (97), on the one hand, and the examples in (98), on the other, is related to the fact that in (97) the als/dan-phrase is not a complement of the adjective itself, but depends on the presence of the intensifier even/net zo or the comparative/superlative morpheme, whereas the PP op zijn vrouw is directly selected by the adjectives verliefd and gek.
That this suggestion might be on the right track is also suggested by the contrast between the (a)- and (b)-examples in (99), which involve clauses. In the former, the clause dat de bom zal vallen is directly selected by the adjective bang, and cannot be placed in postnominal position. In the latter, on the other hand, the clause dat je er meer van lust depends on the intensifier zo, which is clear from the fact that the clause can only be used if this adverb is present, and in this case the clause can be placed postnominally.
a. | De man | is niet bang | dat | de bom | zal | vallen. | |
the man | is not afraid | that | the bomb | will | fall | ||
'The man doesnʼt fear that the bomb will fall.' |
a'. | * | een bange man dat de bom zal vallen |
b. | De wijn | is zeker | *(zo) lekker | dat | je | er | meer | van | lust. | |
the wine | is certainly | so appetizing | that | you | there | more | of | like | ||
'The wine is certainly so appetizing that one likes more of it.' |
b'. | een | zo lekkere wijn | dat | je | er | meer | van | lust | |
a | so appetizing wine | that | you | there | more | of | like | ||
'a wine, so appetizing that one likes more of it' |
For completeness’ sake, note that there is an alternative “repair" strategy that involves placement of the complete AP in postnominal position, and which can be applied to adjectives with a complement and with a modifier alike, provided that the resulting AP is “heavy" enough. We will discuss cases like these more extensively in Section 6.4.
Section 5.2, sub IV, has shown that, as a general rule, intensifiers do not get the inflectional -e ending. This is illustrated again in (100).
a. | een | zeer | grote | inzet |
b. | * | een | zere | grote | inzet |
a | very | large | dedication |
Further, we have seen that the inflected adjective must be adjacent to the noun; cf. the Head-final Filter in (87). From this, we would correctly predict that the examples in (101a-d) are impossible, regardless of whether the intensifier genoeg'enough' is inflected or not. The only way to express the intended notion is by using an appositive phrase, as in (101e); cf. Section 6.4.
a. | * | een | groot | genoeg | inzet | (om ...) |
b. | ?? | een | groot | genoege | inzet | (om ...) |
c. | * | een | grote | genoeg | inzet | (om ...) |
d. | * | een | grote | genoege | inzet | (om ...) |
a | large | enough | dedication | (to ...) |
e. | een inzet, | groot genoeg om ... | |
a dedication | big enough to ... |
Given the unacceptability of the examples in (101a-d), it is perhaps surprising that the discontinuous intensifying phrase zo ... mogelijk'as ... as possible' can be used in the prenominal attributive position, as is shown in (102b). This possibility is certainly due to the fact that the inflectional ending must appear on mogelijk, and cannot appear on the adjective itself. In passing, observe that (102b) has no appositive alternant: (102e) is ungrammatical.
a. | * | een | zo groot mogelijk | inzet |
b. | een | zo groot mogelijke | inzet |
c. | * | een | zo grote mogelijk | inzet |
d. | * | een | zo grote mogelijke | inzet |
an | as large as.possible | dedication |
e. | * | een inzet, | zo groot | mogelijk |
a dedication | as large | as.possible |
A fully satisfactory explanation for the contrast between the examples in (101) and (102) seems beyond reach at this moment. One observation that may bear on the issue is that mogelijk, but not genoeg, can itself be used as an attributive adjective. But perhaps this is not the right direction given that examples such as (101b), although generally judged unacceptable by native speakers, can be observed both in spontaneous speech and in written language on the internet. A Google search (April 2009) on the string [een groot genoege], for example, resulted in 376 hits, which is of course negligible in comparison to the more than 500,000 hits for the string [een zo groot mogelijke], but perhaps substantial enough to hesitate to dismiss these cases as simple performance errors. That more is going on is also suggested by the fact that for many speakers, the constituent A + genoeg can be used with singular, neuter, indefinite noun phrases, that is, if the attributive ending -e is missing; cf. Van Riemsdijk (1998).
a. | % | een | voor mij | groot genoeg | huis |
b. | * | het | voor mij | groot/grote genoeg | huis |
a/the | for me | big enough | house |
That (103a) is more acceptable than (101b) is also reflected in that our Google search on the string [een groot genoeg] resulted in over 1,500 hits. For comparison, note that the string [een zo groot mogelijk] resulted in less than 200,000 hits.
- 1998Head movement and adjacencyNatural Language and Linguistic Theory16633-678