- Dutch
- Frisian
- Saterfrisian
- Afrikaans
-
- Phonology
- Segment inventory
- Phonotactics
- Phonological processes
- Phonology-morphology interface
- Word stress
- Primary stress in simplex words
- Monomorphemic words
- Diachronic aspects
- Generalizations on stress placement
- Default penultimate stress
- Lexical stress
- The closed penult restriction
- Final closed syllables
- The diphthong restriction
- Superheavy syllables (SHS)
- The three-syllable window
- Segmental restrictions
- Phonetic correlates
- Stress shifts in loanwords
- Quantity-sensitivity
- Secondary stress
- Vowel reduction in unstressed syllables
- Stress in complex words
- Primary stress in simplex words
- Accent & intonation
- Clitics
- Spelling
- Morphology
- Word formation
- Compounding
- Nominal compounds
- Verbal compounds
- Adjectival compounds
- Affixoids
- Coordinative compounds
- Synthetic compounds
- Reduplicative compounds
- Phrase-based compounds
- Elative compounds
- Exocentric compounds
- Linking elements
- Separable complex verbs (SCVs)
- Gapping of complex words
- Particle verbs
- Copulative compounds
- Derivation
- Numerals
- Derivation: inputs and input restrictions
- The meaning of affixes
- Non-native morphology
- Cohering and non-cohering affixes
- Prefixation
- Suffixation
- Nominal suffixation: person nouns
- Conversion
- Pseudo-participles
- Bound forms
- Nouns
- Nominal prefixes
- Nominal suffixes
- -aal and -eel
- -aar
- -aard
- -aat
- -air
- -aris
- -ast
- Diminutives
- -dom
- -een
- -ees
- -el (nominal)
- -elaar
- -enis
- -er (nominal)
- -erd
- -erik
- -es
- -eur
- -euse
- ge...te
- -heid
- -iaan, -aan
- -ief
- -iek
- -ier
- -ier (French)
- -ière
- -iet
- -igheid
- -ij and allomorphs
- -ijn
- -in
- -ing
- -isme
- -ist
- -iteit
- -ling
- -oir
- -oot
- -rice
- -schap
- -schap (de)
- -schap (het)
- -sel
- -st
- -ster
- -t
- -tal
- -te
- -voud
- Verbs
- Adjectives
- Adverbs
- Univerbation
- Neo-classical word formation
- Construction-dependent morphology
- Morphological productivity
- Compounding
- Inflection
- Inflection and derivation
- Allomorphy
- The interface between phonology and morphology
- Word formation
- Syntax
- Preface and acknowledgements
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
- 1 Characterization and classification
- 2 Projection of verb phrases I:Argument structure
- 3 Projection of verb phrases II:Verb frame alternations
- Introduction
- 3.1. Main types
- 3.2. Alternations involving the external argument
- 3.3. Alternations of noun phrases and PPs
- 3.3.1. Dative/PP alternations (dative shift)
- 3.3.1.1. Dative alternation with aan-phrases (recipients)
- 3.3.1.2. Dative alternation with naar-phrases (goals)
- 3.3.1.3. Dative alternation with van-phrases (sources)
- 3.3.1.4. Dative alternation with bij-phrases (possessors)
- 3.3.1.5. Dative alternation with voor-phrases (benefactives)
- 3.3.1.6. Conclusion
- 3.3.1.7. Bibliographical notes
- 3.3.2. Accusative/PP alternations
- 3.3.3. Nominative/PP alternations
- 3.3.1. Dative/PP alternations (dative shift)
- 3.4. Some apparent cases of verb frame alternation
- 3.5. Bibliographical notes
- 4 Projection of verb phrases IIIa:Selection of clauses/verb phrases
- 5 Projection of verb phrases IIIb:Argument and complementive clauses
- Introduction
- 5.1. Finite argument clauses
- 5.2. Infinitival argument clauses
- 5.3. Complementive clauses
- 6 Projection of verb phrases IIIc:Complements of non-main verbs
- 7 Projection of verb phrases IIId:Verb clusters
- 8 Projection of verb phrases IV: Adverbial modification
- 9 Word order in the clause I:General introduction
- 10 Word order in the clause II:Position of the finite verb (verb-first/second)
- 11 Word order in the clause III:Clause-initial position (wh-movement)
- Introduction
- 11.1. The formation of V1- and V2-clauses
- 11.2. Clause-initial position remains (phonetically) empty
- 11.3. Clause-initial position is filled
- 12 Word order in the clause IV:Postverbal field (extraposition)
- 13 Word order in the clause V: Middle field (scrambling)
- 14 Main-clause external elements
- Nouns and Noun Phrases
- 1 Characterization and classification
- 2 Projection of noun phrases I: complementation
- Introduction
- 2.1. General observations
- 2.2. Prepositional and nominal complements
- 2.3. Clausal complements
- 2.4. Bibliographical notes
- 3 Projection of noun phrases II: modification
- Introduction
- 3.1. Restrictive and non-restrictive modifiers
- 3.2. Premodification
- 3.3. Postmodification
- 3.3.1. Adpositional phrases
- 3.3.2. Relative clauses
- 3.3.3. Infinitival clauses
- 3.3.4. A special case: clauses referring to a proposition
- 3.3.5. Adjectival phrases
- 3.3.6. Adverbial postmodification
- 3.4. Bibliographical notes
- 4 Projection of noun phrases III: binominal constructions
- Introduction
- 4.1. Binominal constructions without a preposition
- 4.2. Binominal constructions with a preposition
- 4.3. Bibliographical notes
- 5 Determiners: articles and pronouns
- Introduction
- 5.1. Articles
- 5.2. Pronouns
- 5.3. Bibliographical notes
- 6 Numerals and quantifiers
- 7 Pre-determiners
- Introduction
- 7.1. The universal quantifier al 'all' and its alternants
- 7.2. The pre-determiner heel 'all/whole'
- 7.3. A note on focus particles
- 7.4. Bibliographical notes
- 8 Syntactic uses of noun phrases
- Adjectives and Adjective Phrases
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- 2 Projection of adjective phrases I: Complementation
- 3 Projection of adjective phrases II: Modification
- 4 Projection of adjective phrases III: Comparison
- 5 Attributive use of the adjective phrase
- 6 Predicative use of the adjective phrase
- 7 The partitive genitive construction
- 8 Adverbial use of the adjective phrase
- 9 Participles and infinitives: their adjectival use
- 10 Special constructions
- Adpositions and adpositional phrases
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- Introduction
- 1.1. Characterization of the category adposition
- 1.2. A formal classification of adpositional phrases
- 1.3. A semantic classification of adpositional phrases
- 1.3.1. Spatial adpositions
- 1.3.2. Temporal adpositions
- 1.3.3. Non-spatial/temporal prepositions
- 1.4. Borderline cases
- 1.5. Bibliographical notes
- 2 Projection of adpositional phrases: Complementation
- 3 Projection of adpositional phrases: Modification
- 4 Syntactic uses of the adpositional phrase
- 5 R-pronominalization and R-words
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- Phonology
-
- General
- Phonology
- Segment inventory
- Phonotactics
- Phonological Processes
- Assimilation
- Vowel nasalization
- Syllabic sonorants
- Final devoicing
- Fake geminates
- Vowel hiatus resolution
- Vowel reduction introduction
- Schwa deletion
- Schwa insertion
- /r/-deletion
- d-insertion
- {s/z}-insertion
- t-deletion
- Intrusive stop formation
- Breaking
- Vowel shortening
- h-deletion
- Replacement of the glide w
- Word stress
- Clitics
- Allomorphy
- Orthography of Frisian
- Morphology
- Inflection
- Word formation
- Derivation
- Prefixation
- Infixation
- Suffixation
- Nominal suffixes
- Verbal suffixes
- Adjectival suffixes
- Adverbial suffixes
- Numeral suffixes
- Interjectional suffixes
- Onomastic suffixes
- Conversion
- Compositions
- Derivation
- Syntax
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Unergative and unaccusative subjects
- Evidentiality
- To-infinitival clauses
- Predication and noun incorporation
- Ellipsis
- Imperativus-pro-Infinitivo
- Expression of irrealis
- Embedded Verb Second
- Agreement
- Negation
- Nouns & Noun Phrases
- Classification
- Complementation
- Modification
- Partitive noun constructions
- Referential partitive constructions
- Partitive measure nouns
- Numeral partitive constructions
- Partitive question constructions
- Nominalised quantifiers
- Kind partitives
- Partitive predication with prepositions
- Bare nominal attributions
- Articles and names
- Pronouns
- Quantifiers and (pre)determiners
- Interrogative pronouns
- R-pronouns
- Syntactic uses
- Adjective Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Complementation
- Modification and degree quantification
- Comparison by degree
- Comparative
- Superlative
- Equative
- Attribution
- Agreement
- Attributive adjectives vs. prenominal elements
- Complex adjectives
- Noun ellipsis
- Co-occurring adjectives
- Predication
- Partitive adjective constructions
- Adverbial use
- Participles and infinitives
- Adposition Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Complementation
- Modification
- Intransitive adpositions
- Predication
- Preposition stranding
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
-
- General
- Morphology
- Morphology
- 1 Word formation
- 1.1 Compounding
- 1.1.1 Compounds and their heads
- 1.1.2 Special types of compounds
- 1.1.2.1 Affixoids
- 1.1.2.2 Coordinative compounds
- 1.1.2.3 Synthetic compounds and complex pseudo-participles
- 1.1.2.4 Reduplicative compounds
- 1.1.2.5 Phrase-based compounds
- 1.1.2.6 Elative compounds
- 1.1.2.7 Exocentric compounds
- 1.1.2.8 Linking elements
- 1.1.2.9 Separable Complex Verbs and Particle Verbs
- 1.1.2.10 Noun Incorporation Verbs
- 1.1.2.11 Gapping
- 1.2 Derivation
- 1.3 Minor patterns of word formation
- 1.1 Compounding
- 2 Inflection
- 1 Word formation
- Morphology
- Syntax
- Adjectives and adjective phrases (APs)
- 0 Introduction to the AP
- 1 Characteristics and classification of APs
- 2 Complementation of APs
- 3 Modification and degree quantification of APs
- 4 Comparison by comparative, superlative and equative
- 5 Attribution of APs
- 6 Predication of APs
- 7 The partitive adjective construction
- 8 Adverbial use of APs
- 9 Participles and infinitives as APs
- Nouns and Noun Phrases (NPs)
- 0 Introduction to the NP
- 1 Characteristics and Classification of NPs
- 2 Complementation of NPs
- 3 Modification of NPs
- 3.1 Modification of NP by Determiners and APs
- 3.2 Modification of NP by PP
- 3.3 Modification of NP by adverbial clauses
- 3.4 Modification of NP by possessors
- 3.5 Modification of NP by relative clauses
- 3.6 Modification of NP in a cleft construction
- 3.7 Free relative clauses and selected interrogative clauses
- 4 Partitive noun constructions and constructions related to them
- 4.1 The referential partitive construction
- 4.2 The partitive construction of abstract quantity
- 4.3 The numerical partitive construction
- 4.4 The partitive interrogative construction
- 4.5 Adjectival, nominal and nominalised partitive quantifiers
- 4.6 Kind partitives
- 4.7 Partitive predication with a preposition
- 4.8 Bare nominal attribution
- 5 Articles and names
- 6 Pronouns
- 7 Quantifiers, determiners and predeterminers
- 8 Interrogative pronouns
- 9 R-pronouns and the indefinite expletive
- 10 Syntactic functions of Noun Phrases
- Adpositions and Adpositional Phrases (PPs)
- 0 Introduction to the PP
- 1 Characteristics and classification of PPs
- 2 Complementation of PPs
- 3 Modification of PPs
- 4 Bare (intransitive) adpositions
- 5 Predication of PPs
- 6 Form and distribution of adpositions with respect to staticity and construction type
- 7 Adpositional complements and adverbials
- Verbs and Verb Phrases (VPs)
- 0 Introduction to the VP in Saterland Frisian
- 1 Characteristics and classification of verbs
- 2 Unergative and unaccusative subjects and the auxiliary of the perfect
- 3 Evidentiality in relation to perception and epistemicity
- 4 Types of to-infinitival constituents
- 5 Predication
- 5.1 The auxiliary of being and its selection restrictions
- 5.2 The auxiliary of going and its selection restrictions
- 5.3 The auxiliary of continuation and its selection restrictions
- 5.4 The auxiliary of coming and its selection restrictions
- 5.5 Modal auxiliaries and their selection restrictions
- 5.6 Auxiliaries of body posture and aspect and their selection restrictions
- 5.7 Transitive verbs of predication
- 5.8 The auxiliary of doing used as a semantically empty finite auxiliary
- 5.9 Supplementive predication
- 6 The verbal paradigm, irregularity and suppletion
- 7 Verb Second and the word order in main and embedded clauses
- 8 Various aspects of clause structure
- Adjectives and adjective phrases (APs)
-
- General
- Phonology
- Afrikaans phonology
- Segment inventory
- Overview of Afrikaans vowels
- The diphthongised long vowels /e/, /ø/ and /o/
- The unrounded mid-front vowel /ɛ/
- The unrounded low-central vowel /ɑ/
- The unrounded low-central vowel /a/
- The rounded mid-high back vowel /ɔ/
- The rounded high back vowel /u/
- The rounded and unrounded high front vowels /i/ and /y/
- The unrounded and rounded central vowels /ə/ and /œ/
- The diphthongs /əi/, /œy/ and /œu/
- Overview of Afrikaans consonants
- The bilabial plosives /p/ and /b/
- The alveolar plosives /t/ and /d/
- The velar plosives /k/ and /g/
- The bilabial nasal /m/
- The alveolar nasal /n/
- The velar nasal /ŋ/
- The trill /r/
- The lateral liquid /l/
- The alveolar fricative /s/
- The velar fricative /x/
- The labiodental fricatives /f/ and /v/
- The approximants /ɦ/, /j/ and /ʋ/
- Overview of Afrikaans vowels
- Word stress
- The phonetic properties of stress
- Primary stress on monomorphemic words in Afrikaans
- Background to primary stress in monomorphemes in Afrikaans
- Overview of the Main Stress Rule of Afrikaans
- The short vowels of Afrikaans
- Long vowels in monomorphemes
- Primary stress on diphthongs in monomorphemes
- Exceptions
- Stress shifts in place names
- Stress shift towards word-final position
- Stress pattern of reduplications
- Phonological processes
- Vowel related processes
- Consonant related processes
- Homorganic glide insertion
- Phonology-morphology interface
- Phonotactics
- Morphology
- Syntax
- Afrikaans syntax
- Nouns and noun phrases
- Characteristics of the NP
- Classification of nouns
- Complementation of NPs
- Modification of NPs
- Binominal and partitive constructions
- Referential partitive constructions
- Partitive measure nouns
- Numeral partitive constructions
- Partitive question constructions
- Partitive constructions with nominalised quantifiers
- Partitive predication with prepositions
- Binominal name constructions
- Binominal genitive constructions
- Bare nominal attribution
- Articles and names
- Pronouns
- Quantifiers, determiners and predeterminers
- Syntactic uses of the noun phrase
- Adjectives and adjective phrases
- Characteristics and classification of the AP
- Complementation of APs
- Modification and Degree Quantification of APs
- Comparison by comparative, superlative and equative degree
- Attribution of APs
- Predication of APs
- The partitive adjective construction
- Adverbial use of APs
- Participles and infinitives as adjectives
- Verbs and verb phrases
- Characterisation and classification
- Argument structure
- Verb frame alternations
- Complements of non-main verbs
- Verb clusters
- Complement clauses
- Adverbial modification
- Word order in the clause: Introduction
- Word order in the clause: position of the finite Verb
- Word order in the clause: Clause-initial position
- Word order in the clause: Extraposition and right-dislocation in the postverbal field
- Word order in the middle field
- Emphatic constructions
- Adpositions and adposition phrases
This section discusses some general properties of complementives. We will start by reviewing the construction types that contain a complementive in Subsection I. This is followed in Subsection II by a discussion of the categories that the complementives may have, subsection III continues with the positions that the complementives may occupy within the sentence, that is, whether they can be scrambled, topicalized, etc, subsection IV concludes with a discussion of the co-occurrence restrictions between complementives.
There are three constructions in which complementives are found. The first is the copular construction, illustrated by (160a), in which the complementive is predicated of the subject of the clause. The second is the so-called vinden-construction, illustrated by (160b), in which the complementive is predicated of the accusative argument of the clause: this construction conveys a subjective evaluation of the object by the subject of the clause. The third construction is the resultative construction: if this construction contains an accusative object, as in (160c), it is this object that the complementive is predicated of; if the construction does not contain an accusative noun phrase, as in (160c'), the complementive is predicated of the subject of the clause.
a. | Jan is aardig. | copular construction | |
Jan is nice |
b. | Ik | vind | Jan aardig. | vinden-construction | |
I | consider | Jan nice |
c. | Jan slaat | Peter dood. | resultative construction | |
Jan hits | Peter dead |
c'. | Jan valt | dood. | resultative construction | |
Jan falls | dead |
The examples in (160) all involve a complementive AP. The complementive can, however, also be a PP or a noun phrase. This is illustrated in (161) for the copular construction. Note that traditional grammar strongly opposes the idea that the PP in (161b) is a complementive, and analyzes this PP as an adverbial phrase. We will see in Subsection III, however, that PPs of this type have all the distributional properties of a complementive.
a. | Jan is ziek. | AP-complementive | |
Jan is ill |
b. | Jan is naar Utrecht. | PP-complementive | |
Jan is to Utrecht |
c. | Jan is een schurk. | NP-complementive | |
Jan is a villain |
Example (162a) shows that so-called modal infinitives can also be used as complementives: such infinitives, which behave like adjectival complementives in the relevant respects, are more extensively discussed in A9. The (b)-examples in (162) show that we occasionally also find om + te-infinitives; such infinitival clause often receive an idiomatic interpretation.
a. | Dat boek | is | gemakkelijk | te lezen. | |
that book | is | easy | to read | ||
'That book is easy to read.' |
b. | De wedstrijd | is niet | [om | over naar huis | te schrijven]. | |
the game | is not | comp | to home | to write | ||
'The game was disappointing.' |
b'. | De baby was [om | op | te vreten]. | |
the baby was comp | down | to gobble | ||
'The baby was lovely.' |
b''. | Het geluid | is [om | gek | van | te worden]. | |
the sound | is comp | crazy | of | to become | ||
'The sound is driving me crazy.' |
The examples in (163) and (164) show that the same types of complementives may occur in the vinden-construction.
a. | Marie vindt | Jan aardig. | AP-complementive | |
Marie considers | Jan nice |
b. | $ | Marie | vindt | Jan onder de maat. | PP-complementive |
Marie | considers | Jan under the measure | |||
'Marie considers Jan not up to the mark/inadequate.' |
c. | Marie vindt | Jan een schurk. | NP-complementive | |
Marie considers | Jan a villain |
a. | Ik | vind | dat boek | gemakkelijk | te lezen. | |
I | consider | that book | easy | to read | ||
'I consider that book easy to read.' |
b. | Ik vind | de wedstrijd | niet | [om | over naar huis | te schrijven]. | |
I consider | the game | not | comp | to home | to write | ||
'I consider the game disappointing.' |
It should be noted, however, that vinden-constructions with a complementive PP are rare and often more or less idiomatic in nature. Examples such as (165a) are possible but not under the intended reading: the verb vinden is instead construed with the meaning "to find", and the PP functions as an adverbial phrase of place: the garden is the place in which Marie found the golden coin. Examples with a directional PP are outright ungrammatical.
a. | # | Marie vond | de gouden munt | in de tuin. |
Marie found | the golden coin | in the garden |
b. | $Marie vindt | Jan naar Utrecht. | |
Marie considers | Jan to Utrecht |
The reason for the unacceptability of the vinden-constructions in (165) does not seem to be syntactic in nature. We noted earlier that the vinden-construction expresses a subjective evaluation of the accusative noun phrase by the subject of the clause, and as a result of this, the complementive must be evaluative in nature: if it denotes a property that can be objectively established, the result is semantically anomalous. The restriction accounts, for example, for the unacceptability of an example such as (166a) and for the fact that (166) is only possible if construed with an added evaluative meaning aspect. Given that the PPs in the examples in (165) also lack the required subjective contents, the unacceptability of these examples under the intended reading does not come as a surprise.
a. | * | Marie vindt | Jan dood. |
Marie considers | Jan dead |
b. | Marie vindt | Jan | een man. | |
Marie considers | Jan | a man | ||
'Marie considers Jan a true/prototypical/... man.' |
In (167), finally, we give some examples of the resultative construction. Example (167c) shows that, for some unclear reason, complementives cannot be nominal in this construction; see Section 2.2.3, sub IA.
a. | Marie slaat | Jan dood. | AP-complementive | |
Marie beats | Jan dead |
b. | Marie gooit | Jan uit de trein. | PP-complementive | |
Marie throws | Jan out.of the train |
c. | * | Marie slaat | Jan een invalide. | NP-complementive |
Marie beats | Jan an invalid |
Although noun phrases cannot be used as a complementive in the resultative construction, it is often possible to express the intended meaning by making use of an adpositional phrase introduced by tot; example (168a) expresses that the spinach changes into a pulp as a result of the cutting event and (168b) expresses that Jan is becoming a knight as the result of the action of the king. This construction is discussed more extensively in Section P4.2.1.2, sub II.
a. | Jan hakt | de spinazie | tot moes. | |
Jan cuts | the spinach | to pulp |
b. | De koning | slaat | Jan tot ridder. | |
the king | hits | Jan to knight | ||
'The king raises Jan to the peerage.' |
It is often claimed that verbal particles are also complementives; cf. Den Dikken (1995). These particles are then analyzed as intransitive adpositions, that is, instances of PP-complementives. Some examples with the particle weg'away' are given in (169). As expected on the basis of the findings in (165), the particle weg cannot be used in the vinden-construction: again this is due to the lack of subjective content. Since we will not extensively discuss verbal particles here, we refer the reader to Section P1.2.4 for a more detailed discussion.
a. | Jan is weg. | |
Jan is away |
b. | $ | Marie vindt | Jan weg. |
Marie considers | Jan away |
c. | Marie stuurt | Jan weg. | |
Marie sends | Jan away |
Although Dutch has a relatively free word order, this subsection shows that the position of the complementive is relatively fixed; complementives occur left-adjacent to the verb(s) in clause-final position, unless they are topicalized or wh-moved.
The examples in (170) show that complementives normally occupy a position to the left of the verb(s) in clause-final position; placement of the complement in postverbal position leads to ungrammaticality. Recall from Subsection II that traditional grammar strongly opposes the idea that the PP in (170b) is a complementive, and analyzes it as an adverbial phrase. The fact that it must precede the clause-final verb shows, however, that it behaves as a complementive; see e.g Van den Berg (1978) and Mulder and Wehrmann (1989).
a. | dat | Marie Jan waarschijnlijk | <dood> | slaat <*dood>. | |
that | Marie Jan probably | dead | beat | ||
'that Marie probably hits Jan to death.' |
b. | dat | Peter | de hond | met de auto | <naar Utrecht> | brengt <*?naar Utrecht>. | |
that | Peter | the dog | with the car | to Utrecht | brings | ||
'that Peter brings the dog to Utrecht by car.' |
c. | dat | Marie Peter nog | steeds | <een schurk> | vindt <*een schurk>. | |
that | Marie Peter prt | still | a villain | considers | ||
'that Marie still considers Peter a villain.' |
The examples in (171) show that something similar holds for embedded clauses with two (or more) verbs. The complementive is normally placed to the left of the clause-final verb cluster, although the percentage signs indicate that some speakers also allow the complementive to permeate the verb cluster. Placement of the complementive after the verb cluster is unacceptable for all speakers.
a. | dat | Marie Jan waarschijnlijk | <dood> | zal <%dood> | slaan <*dood>. | |
that | Marie Jan probably | dead | will | beat |
b. | dat P. de hond met de auto <naar Utrecht> | zal <%naar U> | brengen <*naar U>. | |
that P. the dog with the car to Utrecht | will | bring |
c. | dat M. P. altijd <een schurk> | heeft <%een schurk > | gevonden <*een schurk>. | |
that M. P. always a villain | has | considered |
Permeation of the verb cluster is especially common for speakers of various southern varieties of Dutch, although this is also a marginally acceptable option for some northern speakers if the complementive consists of a single word; such speakers do allow (171a) while rejecting (171b&c). If the complementive is a verbal particle like weg, all speakers allow the complementive in between the verbs.
dat | Marie Jan | <weg> | heeft <weg> | gestuurd <*weg>. | ||
that | Marie Jan | away | has | sent | ||
'that Marie has sent away Jan.' |
The examples in (170) have shown that complementives normally precede the verb(s) in clause-final position. The examples in (173) show that this statement must be made more precise: the complementive must normally be immediately left-adjacent to the verb(s) in clause-final position. In other words, complementives cannot be scrambled across the adverbial phrases in the middle field of the clause.
a. | dat | Marie Jan <*dood> | waarschijnlijk <dood> | slaat. | |
that | Marie Jan dead | probably | beats | ||
'that Marie probably hits Jan to death.' |
b. | dat | Peter | de hond | <*naar Utrecht> | met de auto <naar Utrecht> | brengt. | |
that | Peter | the dog | to Utrecht | with the car | brings | ||
'that Peter brings the dog to Utrecht by car.' |
c. | dat | Marie Peter <*een schurk> | nog | steeds <een schurk> | vindt. | |
that | Marie Peter a villain | prt | still | considers | ||
'that Marie still considers Peter a villain.' |
When the complementive competes with some other element for the position left-adjacent to the verb(s) in clause-final position, however, a limited amount of word order variation may arise. This especially holds for resultative constructions like (174a&b), in which the complementive is in competition with a stranded preposition, which normally also occupies the position left-adjacent to the verb(s); we refer the reader to Section A6.2.2, sub III for a more extensive discussion of some factors that may affect the outcome of this competition. Note that we have not been able to construct examples with a nominal complementive, which is due to the fact that these do not appear in the resultative construction. Example (174c) shows that particles behave like full PPs.
a. | dat | Marie Jan met een knuppel | dood | slaat. | |
that | Marie Jan with a bat | dead | beats | ||
'that Marie is beating Jan to death with a bat.' |
a'. | dat Marie er Jan <mee> dood <mee> slaat. |
b. | dat | Peter | de hond | met de auto | naar Utrecht | brengt. | |
that | Peter | the dog | with the car | to Utrecht | brings | ||
'that Peter brings the dog to Utrecht by car.' |
b'. | dat Jan er de hond <mee> naar Utrecht <mee> brengt. |
c. | dat | Marie Jan met een knuppel | weg | jaagde. | |
that | Marie Jan with a bat | away | chased | ||
'that Marie chased Jan away with a bat.' |
c'. | dat Marie er Jan <mee> weg <mee> jaagde. |
Although complementives are normally placed left-adjacent to the verb(s) in clause-final position, they can also occur in sentence-initial position as the result of topicalization or wh-movement. Some examples are given in (175) and (176).
a. | Doodi | heeft | Marie hem ti | geslagen. | |
dead | has | Marie him | beaten |
b. | [Naar Utrecht]i | heeft | Jan de hond ti | gebracht. | |
to Utrecht | has | Jan the dog | brought |
c. | [Een schurk]i | vindt | Marie | Peter nog | steeds ti. | |
a villain | considers | Marie | Peter prt | still |
a. | [Hoe aardig]i | vindt | Marie | hem ti? | |
how kind | considers | Marie | him |
b. | [In welke la]i | heeft | Jan het mes | gelegd ti? | |
into which drawer | has | Jan the knife | put | ||
'Into which drawer did Jan put the knife?' |
c. | [Wat voor type mens]i | vind | je | Peter ti? | |
what kind of person | consider | you | Peter | ||
'What kind of person do you think Peter is?' |
Examples (177a&b) show that the verb zetten'to put' can take either an adjectival or an adpositional complementive. Example (177c) cannot, however, be interpreted in such a way that both op straatandklaar act as complementives; it is only the adjective that is interpreted in that way. The PP op straat must be interpreted as a locational adverbial phrase, which can be made clear by means of the adverbial en doet dat test: the fact that (177b) cannot be paraphrased by means of (177b') shows that the PP op straat does not function as an adverbial phrase in contrast to what is the case with the same PP in (177c).
a. | Jan zet | de vuilnisemmer | klaar. | |
Jan puts | the garbage can | ready |
b. | Jan | zet | de vuilnisemmer | op straat. | |
Jan | puts | the garbage can | in the.street |
b'. | * | Jan zet | de vuilnisemmer | en | hij | doet | dat | op straat. |
Jan puts | the garbage can | and | he | does | that | in the.street |
c. | # | Jan zet de vuilnisemmer op straat klaar. |
c'. | Jan zet de vuilnisemmer klaar en hij doet dat op straat. |
The discussion of the examples in (177) suggests that a clause can contain at most one complementive. If the suggestion from Subsection II that particles of particle verbs like opbellen'to phone' are complementives is on the right track, this constraint on the number of complementives immediately accounts for the fact that particle verbs are incompatible with complementives.
a. | Jan belt | zijn ouders | op. | |
Jan phones | his parents | prt. | ||
'Jan phones his parents.' |
b. | Jan belt | zijn ouders | arm. | |
Jan phones | his parents | poor | ||
'Jan phones so much that he makes his parents poor.' |
c. | * | Jan belt zijn ouders arm op/op arm. |
Examples like those in (179) seem to be a problem for the claim that a clause can contain at most one complementive; examples (179a&b) show that the verb leggen'to put' can take either a particle or an adpositional phrase as a complementive, and example (179c) shows that both can appear simultaneously. It should be noted, however, that the prepositional phrases in (179a) and (179c) exhibit different behavior when it comes to their placement in the clause, subsection III has established that complementives can never follow the verb(s) in clause-final position, and example (179a) shows that the PP op de tafel is a well-behaved complementive in this respect. The PP in (179c), on the other hand, can readily follow the verb in clause-final position, and we should therefore conclude that it does not function as a complementive if the particle is present. This conclusion is also supported by the fact illustrated in (179d) that the PP can also scramble across the object if the particle is present. See Broekhuis (1992) and Den Dikken (1995) for two competing analyses of such examples.
a. | dat | Jan het boek | <op de tafel> | legde <*op de tafel>. | |
that | Jan the book | on the table | put | ||
'that Jan put the book on the table.' |
b. | dat | Jan het boek | neer | legde. | |
that | Jan the book | down | put | ||
'that Jan put the book down.' |
c. | dat | Jan het boek | <op de tafel> | neer | legde <op de tafel>. | |
that | Jan the book | on the table | down | put | ||
'that Jan put the book down on the table.' |
d. | dat | Jan | op de tafel | het boek | ??(neer) | legde. | |
that | Jan | on the table | the book | down | put | ||
'that Jan put the book down on the table.' |
The examples in (180) show that we can find a similar phenomenon with verbs prefixed with be-. The resultative example in (180a) shows that complementive tot-phrases normally precede the verb in clause-final position. However, if the tot-phrase is selected by a verb prefixed with be-, it can either precede or follow the verb. This suggests that prefixes like be-, ver-and ont- resemble particles like neer in (179) in that they also function syntactically as complementives; see Section 3.3.2, sub B, for a discussion of a proposal of this sort.
a. | dat | de koning | Jan <tot ridder> | heeft | geslagen <*tot ridder>. | |
that | the king | Jan to knight | has | hit | ||
'that the king made Jan a knight.' |
b. | dat | de koning | Jan <tot adviseur> | heeft | benoemd <tot adviseur>. | |
that | the king | Jan to advisor | has | appointed | ||
'that the king has appointed Jan as advisor.' |
Subsection I has shown that there are three types of complementive constructions: the copula, vinden- and resultative constructions, subsection II has further shown that complementives can be adjectival, prepositional or nominal in nature, although it should be noted that nominal complementives are not possible in resultative constructions; their place is taken by tot-PPs, subsection III has shown that complementives are normally left-adjacent to the verb(s) in clause-final position, although speakers of certain southern varieties of Dutch also allow them in verb clusters; placement of the complementive after the verb cluster is always impossible. Scrambling of complementives is normally not possible but they can readily undergo wh-movement and topicalization, subsection IV, finally, has shown that a clause can contain at most one complementive.
- 1978Fokus presuppositie en NP-preposingDe Nieuwe Taalgids71212-222
- 1992Chain-government: issues in Dutch syntaxThe Hague, Holland Academic GraphicsUniversity of Amsterdam/HILThesis
- 1995Particles: on the syntax of verb-particle, triadic, and causative constructionsOxford studies in comparative syntaxNew York/OxfordOxford University Press
- 1995Particles: on the syntax of verb-particle, triadic, and causative constructionsOxford studies in comparative syntaxNew York/OxfordOxford University Press
- 1989Locational verbs as unaccusativesBennis, Hans & Kemenade, Ans van (eds.)Linguistics in the Netherlands 1989Dordrecht111-122