- Dutch
- Frisian
- Saterfrisian
- Afrikaans
-
- Phonology
- Segment inventory
- Phonotactics
- Phonological processes
- Phonology-morphology interface
- Word stress
- Primary stress in simplex words
- Monomorphemic words
- Diachronic aspects
- Generalizations on stress placement
- Default penultimate stress
- Lexical stress
- The closed penult restriction
- Final closed syllables
- The diphthong restriction
- Superheavy syllables (SHS)
- The three-syllable window
- Segmental restrictions
- Phonetic correlates
- Stress shifts in loanwords
- Quantity-sensitivity
- Secondary stress
- Vowel reduction in unstressed syllables
- Stress in complex words
- Primary stress in simplex words
- Accent & intonation
- Clitics
- Spelling
- Morphology
- Word formation
- Compounding
- Nominal compounds
- Verbal compounds
- Adjectival compounds
- Affixoids
- Coordinative compounds
- Synthetic compounds
- Reduplicative compounds
- Phrase-based compounds
- Elative compounds
- Exocentric compounds
- Linking elements
- Separable complex verbs (SCVs)
- Gapping of complex words
- Particle verbs
- Copulative compounds
- Derivation
- Numerals
- Derivation: inputs and input restrictions
- The meaning of affixes
- Non-native morphology
- Cohering and non-cohering affixes
- Prefixation
- Suffixation
- Nominal suffixation: person nouns
- Conversion
- Pseudo-participles
- Bound forms
- Nouns
- Nominal prefixes
- Nominal suffixes
- -aal and -eel
- -aar
- -aard
- -aat
- -air
- -aris
- -ast
- Diminutives
- -dom
- -een
- -ees
- -el (nominal)
- -elaar
- -enis
- -er (nominal)
- -erd
- -erik
- -es
- -eur
- -euse
- ge...te
- -heid
- -iaan, -aan
- -ief
- -iek
- -ier
- -ier (French)
- -ière
- -iet
- -igheid
- -ij and allomorphs
- -ijn
- -in
- -ing
- -isme
- -ist
- -iteit
- -ling
- -oir
- -oot
- -rice
- -schap
- -schap (de)
- -schap (het)
- -sel
- -st
- -ster
- -t
- -tal
- -te
- -voud
- Verbs
- Adjectives
- Adverbs
- Univerbation
- Neo-classical word formation
- Construction-dependent morphology
- Morphological productivity
- Compounding
- Inflection
- Inflection and derivation
- Allomorphy
- The interface between phonology and morphology
- Word formation
- Syntax
- Preface and acknowledgements
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
- 1 Characterization and classification
- 2 Projection of verb phrases I:Argument structure
- 3 Projection of verb phrases II:Verb frame alternations
- Introduction
- 3.1. Main types
- 3.2. Alternations involving the external argument
- 3.3. Alternations of noun phrases and PPs
- 3.3.1. Dative/PP alternations (dative shift)
- 3.3.1.1. Dative alternation with aan-phrases (recipients)
- 3.3.1.2. Dative alternation with naar-phrases (goals)
- 3.3.1.3. Dative alternation with van-phrases (sources)
- 3.3.1.4. Dative alternation with bij-phrases (possessors)
- 3.3.1.5. Dative alternation with voor-phrases (benefactives)
- 3.3.1.6. Conclusion
- 3.3.1.7. Bibliographical notes
- 3.3.2. Accusative/PP alternations
- 3.3.3. Nominative/PP alternations
- 3.3.1. Dative/PP alternations (dative shift)
- 3.4. Some apparent cases of verb frame alternation
- 3.5. Bibliographical notes
- 4 Projection of verb phrases IIIa:Selection of clauses/verb phrases
- 5 Projection of verb phrases IIIb:Argument and complementive clauses
- Introduction
- 5.1. Finite argument clauses
- 5.2. Infinitival argument clauses
- 5.3. Complementive clauses
- 6 Projection of verb phrases IIIc:Complements of non-main verbs
- 7 Projection of verb phrases IIId:Verb clusters
- 8 Projection of verb phrases IV: Adverbial modification
- 9 Word order in the clause I:General introduction
- 10 Word order in the clause II:Position of the finite verb (verb-first/second)
- 11 Word order in the clause III:Clause-initial position (wh-movement)
- Introduction
- 11.1. The formation of V1- and V2-clauses
- 11.2. Clause-initial position remains (phonetically) empty
- 11.3. Clause-initial position is filled
- 12 Word order in the clause IV:Postverbal field (extraposition)
- 13 Word order in the clause V: Middle field (scrambling)
- 14 Main-clause external elements
- Nouns and Noun Phrases
- 1 Characterization and classification
- 2 Projection of noun phrases I: complementation
- Introduction
- 2.1. General observations
- 2.2. Prepositional and nominal complements
- 2.3. Clausal complements
- 2.4. Bibliographical notes
- 3 Projection of noun phrases II: modification
- Introduction
- 3.1. Restrictive and non-restrictive modifiers
- 3.2. Premodification
- 3.3. Postmodification
- 3.3.1. Adpositional phrases
- 3.3.2. Relative clauses
- 3.3.3. Infinitival clauses
- 3.3.4. A special case: clauses referring to a proposition
- 3.3.5. Adjectival phrases
- 3.3.6. Adverbial postmodification
- 3.4. Bibliographical notes
- 4 Projection of noun phrases III: binominal constructions
- Introduction
- 4.1. Binominal constructions without a preposition
- 4.2. Binominal constructions with a preposition
- 4.3. Bibliographical notes
- 5 Determiners: articles and pronouns
- Introduction
- 5.1. Articles
- 5.2. Pronouns
- 5.3. Bibliographical notes
- 6 Numerals and quantifiers
- 7 Pre-determiners
- Introduction
- 7.1. The universal quantifier al 'all' and its alternants
- 7.2. The pre-determiner heel 'all/whole'
- 7.3. A note on focus particles
- 7.4. Bibliographical notes
- 8 Syntactic uses of noun phrases
- Adjectives and Adjective Phrases
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- 2 Projection of adjective phrases I: Complementation
- 3 Projection of adjective phrases II: Modification
- 4 Projection of adjective phrases III: Comparison
- 5 Attributive use of the adjective phrase
- 6 Predicative use of the adjective phrase
- 7 The partitive genitive construction
- 8 Adverbial use of the adjective phrase
- 9 Participles and infinitives: their adjectival use
- 10 Special constructions
- Adpositions and adpositional phrases
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- Introduction
- 1.1. Characterization of the category adposition
- 1.2. A formal classification of adpositional phrases
- 1.3. A semantic classification of adpositional phrases
- 1.3.1. Spatial adpositions
- 1.3.2. Temporal adpositions
- 1.3.3. Non-spatial/temporal prepositions
- 1.4. Borderline cases
- 1.5. Bibliographical notes
- 2 Projection of adpositional phrases: Complementation
- 3 Projection of adpositional phrases: Modification
- 4 Syntactic uses of the adpositional phrase
- 5 R-pronominalization and R-words
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- Phonology
-
- General
- Phonology
- Segment inventory
- Phonotactics
- Phonological Processes
- Assimilation
- Vowel nasalization
- Syllabic sonorants
- Final devoicing
- Fake geminates
- Vowel hiatus resolution
- Vowel reduction introduction
- Schwa deletion
- Schwa insertion
- /r/-deletion
- d-insertion
- {s/z}-insertion
- t-deletion
- Intrusive stop formation
- Breaking
- Vowel shortening
- h-deletion
- Replacement of the glide w
- Word stress
- Clitics
- Allomorphy
- Orthography of Frisian
- Morphology
- Inflection
- Word formation
- Derivation
- Prefixation
- Infixation
- Suffixation
- Nominal suffixes
- Verbal suffixes
- Adjectival suffixes
- Adverbial suffixes
- Numeral suffixes
- Interjectional suffixes
- Onomastic suffixes
- Conversion
- Compositions
- Derivation
- Syntax
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Unergative and unaccusative subjects
- Evidentiality
- To-infinitival clauses
- Predication and noun incorporation
- Ellipsis
- Imperativus-pro-Infinitivo
- Expression of irrealis
- Embedded Verb Second
- Agreement
- Negation
- Nouns & Noun Phrases
- Classification
- Complementation
- Modification
- Partitive noun constructions
- Referential partitive constructions
- Partitive measure nouns
- Numeral partitive constructions
- Partitive question constructions
- Nominalised quantifiers
- Kind partitives
- Partitive predication with prepositions
- Bare nominal attributions
- Articles and names
- Pronouns
- Quantifiers and (pre)determiners
- Interrogative pronouns
- R-pronouns
- Syntactic uses
- Adjective Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Complementation
- Modification and degree quantification
- Comparison by degree
- Comparative
- Superlative
- Equative
- Attribution
- Agreement
- Attributive adjectives vs. prenominal elements
- Complex adjectives
- Noun ellipsis
- Co-occurring adjectives
- Predication
- Partitive adjective constructions
- Adverbial use
- Participles and infinitives
- Adposition Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Complementation
- Modification
- Intransitive adpositions
- Predication
- Preposition stranding
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
-
- General
- Morphology
- Morphology
- 1 Word formation
- 1.1 Compounding
- 1.1.1 Compounds and their heads
- 1.1.2 Special types of compounds
- 1.1.2.1 Affixoids
- 1.1.2.2 Coordinative compounds
- 1.1.2.3 Synthetic compounds and complex pseudo-participles
- 1.1.2.4 Reduplicative compounds
- 1.1.2.5 Phrase-based compounds
- 1.1.2.6 Elative compounds
- 1.1.2.7 Exocentric compounds
- 1.1.2.8 Linking elements
- 1.1.2.9 Separable Complex Verbs and Particle Verbs
- 1.1.2.10 Noun Incorporation Verbs
- 1.1.2.11 Gapping
- 1.2 Derivation
- 1.3 Minor patterns of word formation
- 1.1 Compounding
- 2 Inflection
- 1 Word formation
- Morphology
- Syntax
- Adjectives and adjective phrases (APs)
- 0 Introduction to the AP
- 1 Characteristics and classification of APs
- 2 Complementation of APs
- 3 Modification and degree quantification of APs
- 4 Comparison by comparative, superlative and equative
- 5 Attribution of APs
- 6 Predication of APs
- 7 The partitive adjective construction
- 8 Adverbial use of APs
- 9 Participles and infinitives as APs
- Nouns and Noun Phrases (NPs)
- 0 Introduction to the NP
- 1 Characteristics and Classification of NPs
- 2 Complementation of NPs
- 3 Modification of NPs
- 3.1 Modification of NP by Determiners and APs
- 3.2 Modification of NP by PP
- 3.3 Modification of NP by adverbial clauses
- 3.4 Modification of NP by possessors
- 3.5 Modification of NP by relative clauses
- 3.6 Modification of NP in a cleft construction
- 3.7 Free relative clauses and selected interrogative clauses
- 4 Partitive noun constructions and constructions related to them
- 4.1 The referential partitive construction
- 4.2 The partitive construction of abstract quantity
- 4.3 The numerical partitive construction
- 4.4 The partitive interrogative construction
- 4.5 Adjectival, nominal and nominalised partitive quantifiers
- 4.6 Kind partitives
- 4.7 Partitive predication with a preposition
- 4.8 Bare nominal attribution
- 5 Articles and names
- 6 Pronouns
- 7 Quantifiers, determiners and predeterminers
- 8 Interrogative pronouns
- 9 R-pronouns and the indefinite expletive
- 10 Syntactic functions of Noun Phrases
- Adpositions and Adpositional Phrases (PPs)
- 0 Introduction to the PP
- 1 Characteristics and classification of PPs
- 2 Complementation of PPs
- 3 Modification of PPs
- 4 Bare (intransitive) adpositions
- 5 Predication of PPs
- 6 Form and distribution of adpositions with respect to staticity and construction type
- 7 Adpositional complements and adverbials
- Verbs and Verb Phrases (VPs)
- 0 Introduction to the VP in Saterland Frisian
- 1 Characteristics and classification of verbs
- 2 Unergative and unaccusative subjects and the auxiliary of the perfect
- 3 Evidentiality in relation to perception and epistemicity
- 4 Types of to-infinitival constituents
- 5 Predication
- 5.1 The auxiliary of being and its selection restrictions
- 5.2 The auxiliary of going and its selection restrictions
- 5.3 The auxiliary of continuation and its selection restrictions
- 5.4 The auxiliary of coming and its selection restrictions
- 5.5 Modal auxiliaries and their selection restrictions
- 5.6 Auxiliaries of body posture and aspect and their selection restrictions
- 5.7 Transitive verbs of predication
- 5.8 The auxiliary of doing used as a semantically empty finite auxiliary
- 5.9 Supplementive predication
- 6 The verbal paradigm, irregularity and suppletion
- 7 Verb Second and the word order in main and embedded clauses
- 8 Various aspects of clause structure
- Adjectives and adjective phrases (APs)
-
- General
- Phonology
- Afrikaans phonology
- Segment inventory
- Overview of Afrikaans vowels
- The diphthongised long vowels /e/, /ø/ and /o/
- The unrounded mid-front vowel /ɛ/
- The unrounded low-central vowel /ɑ/
- The unrounded low-central vowel /a/
- The rounded mid-high back vowel /ɔ/
- The rounded high back vowel /u/
- The rounded and unrounded high front vowels /i/ and /y/
- The unrounded and rounded central vowels /ə/ and /œ/
- The diphthongs /əi/, /œy/ and /œu/
- Overview of Afrikaans consonants
- The bilabial plosives /p/ and /b/
- The alveolar plosives /t/ and /d/
- The velar plosives /k/ and /g/
- The bilabial nasal /m/
- The alveolar nasal /n/
- The velar nasal /ŋ/
- The trill /r/
- The lateral liquid /l/
- The alveolar fricative /s/
- The velar fricative /x/
- The labiodental fricatives /f/ and /v/
- The approximants /ɦ/, /j/ and /ʋ/
- Overview of Afrikaans vowels
- Word stress
- The phonetic properties of stress
- Primary stress on monomorphemic words in Afrikaans
- Background to primary stress in monomorphemes in Afrikaans
- Overview of the Main Stress Rule of Afrikaans
- The short vowels of Afrikaans
- Long vowels in monomorphemes
- Primary stress on diphthongs in monomorphemes
- Exceptions
- Stress shifts in place names
- Stress shift towards word-final position
- Stress pattern of reduplications
- Phonological processes
- Vowel related processes
- Consonant related processes
- Homorganic glide insertion
- Phonology-morphology interface
- Phonotactics
- Morphology
- Syntax
- Afrikaans syntax
- Nouns and noun phrases
- Characteristics of the NP
- Classification of nouns
- Complementation of NPs
- Modification of NPs
- Binominal and partitive constructions
- Referential partitive constructions
- Partitive measure nouns
- Numeral partitive constructions
- Partitive question constructions
- Partitive constructions with nominalised quantifiers
- Partitive predication with prepositions
- Binominal name constructions
- Binominal genitive constructions
- Bare nominal attribution
- Articles and names
- Pronouns
- Quantifiers, determiners and predeterminers
- Syntactic uses of the noun phrase
- Adjectives and adjective phrases
- Characteristics and classification of the AP
- Complementation of APs
- Modification and Degree Quantification of APs
- Comparison by comparative, superlative and equative degree
- Attribution of APs
- Predication of APs
- The partitive adjective construction
- Adverbial use of APs
- Participles and infinitives as adjectives
- Verbs and verb phrases
- Characterisation and classification
- Argument structure
- Verb frame alternations
- Complements of non-main verbs
- Verb clusters
- Complement clauses
- Adverbial modification
- Word order in the clause: Introduction
- Word order in the clause: position of the finite Verb
- Word order in the clause: Clause-initial position
- Word order in the clause: Extraposition and right-dislocation in the postverbal field
- Word order in the middle field
- Emphatic constructions
- Adpositions and adposition phrases
A clitic and its host word form one phonological word. The phonological evidence testifying to this word-status is the subject of this topic. It is concerned with syllabification and resyllabification facts, the occurrence of syllabic sonorant consonants, and the uninterruptability of the combination of host and clitic.
The assertion that a clitic and its host word form one phonological word presupposes that there are criteria for establishing the clitichood of a function word and, by the same token, the phonological wordhood of the combination of a function word and its host word. The question is therefore when the latter combination can be said to form a phonological word or, put differently, when a function word can be said to show clitic behaviour. As to this, Gussenhoven (1985-1986:180) formulates the, slightly rephrased, criterion:
Criterion for the clitichood of a function word (after Gussenhoven (1985-1986:180)) | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
A function word behaves as a clitic if, together with its host word, it forms a unit for which phonological conditions hold which do not hold for combinations of two non-function words [translated from Dutch]. |
It does not seem too far-fetched to equate the unit for which phonological conditions hold which do not hold for combinations of two non-function words with the phonological word.
Taking the above criterion as a point of departure, there appears to be evidence for the clitic behaviour of function words in Frisian. In the first place, there is evidence based on syllabification. Take the examples below:
Examples of the combination of a preposition and the definite article it 'the (with neuter nouns)' | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
a. | Oan it dak | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
on the roof | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
On the roof | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Oer it wetter | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
over the water | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Across the water | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
b. | Op it dak | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
on the roof | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
On the roof | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Fan it wetter | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
of the water | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Of the water |
The phonological word is considered to be the domain of syllabification. In the examples in (2a), the article it /ət/ the (with neuter nouns) makes up a single phonological word with, viz. cliticizes onto, the prepositions oan /oən/ on and oer /uər/ over by which it is preceded. The syllabification of oan it /oən ət/ and oer it /uər ət/ is as in the example below:
The syllabification of oan it /oən ət/ and oer it /uər ət/ | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
[(oə)(nət)] | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
[(uə)(rət)] |
The final segments of the preposition − /n/ and /r/− form the onset of the right-most syllable, in line with the demand that a word-internal schwa syllable should have an onset (see Schwa). Since these prepositions have a long vocalic sequence here − the centring diphthongs /oə/ and /uə/−, the left-most syllable is in line with the Rhyme Constraint, according to which the rhyme of a word-internal syllable must occupy two structural positions. As to syllabification then oan it (dak) and oer it (wetter) behave just like the inflected words toanen /toən+ən/ [(toə)(nən)] tones and boeren /buər+ən/ [(buə)(rən)] farmers.
The syllabification of op it and fan it in (2b) is as below:
The syllabification of op it /op ət/ and fan it /fɔn ət/ | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
[(op)(pət)] | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
[(fɔn)(nət)] |
Due to short vowels of the prepositions, their final segments − /p/ and /n/− end up as ambisyllabic, that is, as both the coda of the first and the onset of the second syllable, simultaneously satisfying the onset demand for word-internal schwa syllables and the Rhyme Constraint. In this respect, the above sequences resemble the inflected words koppen /kop+ən/ [(kop)(pən)] cups and sokken /sɔk+ən/ [(sɔk)(kən)] socks.
Of course, the above also holds for other combinations than those of a preposition and an article. Finite verbs and personal pronouns are a case in point. Take (ik) seach 'em /sɪəɣ əm/ (I) saw him, which is syllabified as [(sɪə)(ɣəm)], just like the plural form seagen /sɪəɣə+ən/ saws is syllabified as [(sɪə)(ɣən)].
Final Devoicing, in the second place, also gives us a clue. Its effect on the realization of word-final plosives in the combination of a finite verb and a personal pronoun are exemplified below:
Examples of the effect of Final Devoicing on the realization of word-final plosives in the combination of a finite verb and a personal pronoun | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
ried ek | /riəd ək/ | [riətək] | [*riədək] | drove I | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
hold ek | /ho:d ək/ | [ho:tək] | [*ho:dək] | held I |
It seems to be clear that the final obstruents of the above verbs have undergone Final Devoicing. However, the syllabification of ried ek and hold ek − [(riə)(tək)] and [(ho:)(tək)], respectively − is such that verb-final /d/ ends up as the onset of the right-hand syllable, a position in which it cannot be devoiced. Devoicing thus must have taken effect here at a stage where /d/ was in coda position, i.e., when the personal pronoun ek /ək/ I had not yet been amalgamated with the verb. If the phonological word is the domain of syllabification, it is also the domain of resyllabification. Invoking resyllabification here seems to be the only way to reconcile /d/'s underlying voicedness, its devoicing in coda and its voicelessness in onset position. Devoiced /d/ in onset position thus also testifies to the the clitic behaviour of function words. See Booij (1995:174-176) for the same phenomenon in Dutch.
bid ek /bɪd ək/ pray I is realized as [bɪtək], not as [*bɪdək]. Due to the short vowel of bid, bid ek is syllabified as [(bɪt)(tək)], with an ambisyllabic [t]. Before fusing with ek /ək/ I, the final /d/ of bid /bɪd/ pray must have undergone Final Devoicing. In putative [(bɪd)(dək)], /d/ is the coda of the left-most syllable; however, due to its link with the onset of the right-most syllable, it cannot undergo Final Devoicing.
One and the same stem may be operated upon by inflection/derivation on the one and cliticization on the other hand; this may give rise to 'minimal pairs', examples of which are given below:
The primeless examples are realized with [d], the voicing value of which matches that of the /d/ of the underlying representations hoeden /huədən/ cautious, wurden /vød+ən/ words, and rieder /riəd+ər/ advisor. These words are syllabified as [(huə)(dn̩)], [(vød)(dn̩)], and [(riə)(dr̩)], respectively, in line with both the Rhyme Constraint and the onset demand for word-internal schwa syllables; the syllabic sonorant consonants, [n̩] and [r̩], derive from the sequences /ə{n/r}/ (see Evidence that syllabic consonants derive from /ə/ + consonant). Clearly, syllabification takes the underlying representations of these words as its point of departure. The primed examples consist of the same rows of segments as the primeless ones, but they accommodate different morpho-syntactic entities, viz. a sequence of words − like wurd in word a − versus one (complex) word − as with wurden words. If it is assumed that words are juxtaposed at the syntactic level after they have been operated upon by word-level phonology, among which Final Devoicing, it is explained why underlying /d/ is realized as [t] here (see Booij (1995:174-175)) for the same analysis of comparable Dutch cliticization facts). Apart from the difference between [d] and [t], the syllabification of the primed word sequences − [(huə)(tn̩)], [(vøt)(tn̩)], and [(riə)(tr̩)] − equals that of the primeless (complex) words, also with respect to the above demands. As with the examples in (5), devoiced /d/ in onset position is indicative of the the clitic behaviour of function words, in this case in /ən/ a(n) and er /ər/ he.
er /ər/, the clitic counterpart of hy /hi/ or hij /hɛj/ he, only occurs in the position following a finite verb or a subordinating conjunction. If the host word ends in a vowel or a sonorant consonant, er has the variant form der /dər/, as in the following examples:
Dat koe er/der net | ||||||||||||||
that could he not | ||||||||||||||
He couldn't do that |
Dêr kaam er/der oan | ||||||||||||||
there came he on | ||||||||||||||
There he came |
The use of der is obligatory in case the host word ends in /r/. This is nicely illustrated by the verb-subject sequence waard er became he, realized as either [va:tr̩] or [va:dr̩]. The realization with [t] derives from /va:d ər/ through Final Devoicing, cliticization of er onto [va:t], and resyllabification of [t] from the coda of waard to the onset of er (note that the r of waard is purely orthographic). The verb wurde /vød+ə/ to become has the variant form wurre, with the present tense stem stem wur /vør/ and the past tense stem waar /va:r/. The past tense stem, which is not inflected, can only be followed by der, not by er. Since /r/ is deleted when preceding /d/, waar der is realized as [va:dr̩].
Frisian has three voiced-voiceless plosive pairs, viz. /b/ ~ /p/, /d/ ~ /t/, and /ɡ/ ~ /k/. It is, however, only the pair /d/ ~ /t/ which can provide evidence here. The bilabial plosive /b/ has a low rate of occurrence, and there are no verbs of the appropriate classes − weak I and strong/irregular (see Inflectional classes) − with a stem ending in /b/; the pair /b/ ~ /p/ doesn't count, though this is purely accidental. This is different for the pair /ɡ/ ~ /k/; since the velar plosive /ɡ/ only occurs in syllable-initial position (see The dorsal obstruents), the absence of verbs with stem-final /ɡ/ can be accounted for on independent grounds.
The voicing behaviour of the /d/ of /d/-words after a fricative is treated in Progressive Voice Assimilation: function words beginning with /d/). The pattern appears to be fairly clear: a voiceless cluster only occurs if a) the host word ends in a voiceless fricative preceded by a short vowel and b) the /d/-word has the vowel schwa. If the /d/-word has a full vowel, the outcome is a voiced cluster. This also holds in case the host word ends in a voiced fricative preceded by a long vowel, no matter whether the /d/-word has schwa or a full vowel.
In the third place, word-level phonological processes are expected to apply within the combination of a host word and a clitic. The occurrence of syllabic sonorant consonants is a case in point (see Syllabic sonorant consonants). This is exemplified below (see also the cases in (6) above):
Examples of the occurrence of syllabic sonorant consonants in combinations of a host word and a clitic | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
a. | Dat hat er sein | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
/hat ər/ | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
[hatr̩] | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
that has he said | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
He said that | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
b. | Hja rôp 'em deryn | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
/rɔ:p əm/ | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
[rɔ:pm̩] | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
she called him therein | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
She called him in | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
c. | op in kear | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
/op ən/ | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
[opm̩] | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
on a time | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
one day |
The above combinations of host word and clitic are syllabified as [(hat)(tr̩)], [(rɔ:)(pm̩)] and /(op)σ(pm̩)σ/: the final segment of hat /hat/ has, rôp /rɔ:p/ called, and op /op/ on has undergone resyllabification ((8b)) or ambisyllabification ((8a,c)), in line with a) the Rhyme Constraint and b) the demand that a word-internal schwa syllable have an onset (note again that a schwa syllable underlies the syllabic sonorant consonants). The occurrence of the syllabic sonorant consonants therefore also testifies to the the clitic behaviour of function words.
The impersonal pronoun men /mən/ one never shows up with syllabic /n/, as the following examples illustrate:
Like men, the adverb der /dər/ there also has the vowel schwa and an initial consonant, but unlike men it does allow for a syllabic realization of its final sonorant consonant. The above behaviour thus seems typical of men. The fact that men is able to bear stress, despite its vowel, may be responsible for this.
Finally, a phonological word constitutes an uninterruptable unit. This also holds of the combination of a host word and a clitic, as is illustrated below:
Examples of the uninterruptability of a host word plus clitic combination | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
a. | Dat kin ... hy/*er wol dwaan | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
that can ... he all right do | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Let me think ... he can do that | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
b. | Set de ljedder mar tsjin ... de/*'e muorre | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
put the ladder just against ... the wall | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Let me think, just put the ladder against the wall | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
c. | Dat koe, tinkt my alteast, sij/*se sa sein hawwe | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
that could, thinks me at least, she so said have | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
She might have said it like that, at least that's what I think | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
d. | Soks noasket, soe ik tinke, jim/*jem net | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
such a thing pleases, should I think, you (object form, plural, familiar and polite) not | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
You won't be pleased by such a thing, I should think |
If a content word and a function word are separated by a pause ((10a,b)) or a parenthesis ((10c,d)), only the full form of the function word is allowed. As to the above cases this means that hy /hi/ he, de /də/ the, sij /sɛj/ she, and jim /jɪm/ you (object form, plural, familiar and polite) are allowed, to the exclusion of their clitic counterparts er /ər/, 'e /ə/, se /sə/, and jem /jəm/. Cases like those in (10) therefore lend support to the assertion that a clitic and its host word form one phonological word or, the other way around, they testify to the the clitic behaviour of the above function words.
An apposition is a phrase which indicates how a preceding phrase should be interpreted in the context given. As an independent intonational unit, it always has a certain prominence. This may be the reason that a clitic cannot be the 'antecedent' of an apposition, as exemplified below:
Examples of clitics not being able to be the 'antecedent' of an adposition | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Dat koe hij/*er, de heit, him net begripe | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Dat koe hja/*se, de mem, har net begripe | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Ik seach him/*'em, myn buorman, dêr wol stean | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Dat kinne wij/*wi, de erfgenamten, sels wol regelje |
hij /hɛj/ he, hja /ja/ she, him /hɪm/ him, and wij /vɛj/ we are the independent forms of the personal pronouns concerned, whereas er /ər/, se /sə/, 'em /əm/, and wi /vi/ are the clitic counterparts. The former can act as the antecedent of an apposition, the latter cannot.
- 1995The phonology of DutchOxfordOxford University Press
- 1995The phonology of DutchOxfordOxford University Press
- 1985Over de fonologie van Nederlandse cliticaSpektator15180-200
- 1985Over de fonologie van Nederlandse cliticaSpektator15180-200