- Dutch
- Frisian
- Saterfrisian
- Afrikaans
-
- Phonology
- Segment inventory
- Phonotactics
- Phonological processes
- Phonology-morphology interface
- Word stress
- Primary stress in simplex words
- Monomorphemic words
- Diachronic aspects
- Generalizations on stress placement
- Default penultimate stress
- Lexical stress
- The closed penult restriction
- Final closed syllables
- The diphthong restriction
- Superheavy syllables (SHS)
- The three-syllable window
- Segmental restrictions
- Phonetic correlates
- Stress shifts in loanwords
- Quantity-sensitivity
- Secondary stress
- Vowel reduction in unstressed syllables
- Stress in complex words
- Primary stress in simplex words
- Accent & intonation
- Clitics
- Spelling
- Morphology
- Word formation
- Compounding
- Nominal compounds
- Verbal compounds
- Adjectival compounds
- Affixoids
- Coordinative compounds
- Synthetic compounds
- Reduplicative compounds
- Phrase-based compounds
- Elative compounds
- Exocentric compounds
- Linking elements
- Separable complex verbs (SCVs)
- Gapping of complex words
- Particle verbs
- Copulative compounds
- Derivation
- Numerals
- Derivation: inputs and input restrictions
- The meaning of affixes
- Non-native morphology
- Cohering and non-cohering affixes
- Prefixation
- Suffixation
- Nominal suffixation: person nouns
- Conversion
- Pseudo-participles
- Bound forms
- Nouns
- Nominal prefixes
- Nominal suffixes
- -aal and -eel
- -aar
- -aard
- -aat
- -air
- -aris
- -ast
- Diminutives
- -dom
- -een
- -ees
- -el (nominal)
- -elaar
- -enis
- -er (nominal)
- -erd
- -erik
- -es
- -eur
- -euse
- ge...te
- -heid
- -iaan, -aan
- -ief
- -iek
- -ier
- -ier (French)
- -ière
- -iet
- -igheid
- -ij and allomorphs
- -ijn
- -in
- -ing
- -isme
- -ist
- -iteit
- -ling
- -oir
- -oot
- -rice
- -schap
- -schap (de)
- -schap (het)
- -sel
- -st
- -ster
- -t
- -tal
- -te
- -voud
- Verbs
- Adjectives
- Adverbs
- Univerbation
- Neo-classical word formation
- Construction-dependent morphology
- Morphological productivity
- Compounding
- Inflection
- Inflection and derivation
- Allomorphy
- The interface between phonology and morphology
- Word formation
- Syntax
- Preface and acknowledgements
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
- 1 Characterization and classification
- 2 Projection of verb phrases I:Argument structure
- 3 Projection of verb phrases II:Verb frame alternations
- Introduction
- 3.1. Main types
- 3.2. Alternations involving the external argument
- 3.3. Alternations of noun phrases and PPs
- 3.3.1. Dative/PP alternations (dative shift)
- 3.3.1.1. Dative alternation with aan-phrases (recipients)
- 3.3.1.2. Dative alternation with naar-phrases (goals)
- 3.3.1.3. Dative alternation with van-phrases (sources)
- 3.3.1.4. Dative alternation with bij-phrases (possessors)
- 3.3.1.5. Dative alternation with voor-phrases (benefactives)
- 3.3.1.6. Conclusion
- 3.3.1.7. Bibliographical notes
- 3.3.2. Accusative/PP alternations
- 3.3.3. Nominative/PP alternations
- 3.3.1. Dative/PP alternations (dative shift)
- 3.4. Some apparent cases of verb frame alternation
- 3.5. Bibliographical notes
- 4 Projection of verb phrases IIIa:Selection of clauses/verb phrases
- 5 Projection of verb phrases IIIb:Argument and complementive clauses
- Introduction
- 5.1. Finite argument clauses
- 5.2. Infinitival argument clauses
- 5.3. Complementive clauses
- 6 Projection of verb phrases IIIc:Complements of non-main verbs
- 7 Projection of verb phrases IIId:Verb clusters
- 8 Projection of verb phrases IV: Adverbial modification
- 9 Word order in the clause I:General introduction
- 10 Word order in the clause II:Position of the finite verb (verb-first/second)
- 11 Word order in the clause III:Clause-initial position (wh-movement)
- Introduction
- 11.1. The formation of V1- and V2-clauses
- 11.2. Clause-initial position remains (phonetically) empty
- 11.3. Clause-initial position is filled
- 12 Word order in the clause IV:Postverbal field (extraposition)
- 13 Word order in the clause V: Middle field (scrambling)
- 14 Main-clause external elements
- Nouns and Noun Phrases
- 1 Characterization and classification
- 2 Projection of noun phrases I: complementation
- Introduction
- 2.1. General observations
- 2.2. Prepositional and nominal complements
- 2.3. Clausal complements
- 2.4. Bibliographical notes
- 3 Projection of noun phrases II: modification
- Introduction
- 3.1. Restrictive and non-restrictive modifiers
- 3.2. Premodification
- 3.3. Postmodification
- 3.3.1. Adpositional phrases
- 3.3.2. Relative clauses
- 3.3.3. Infinitival clauses
- 3.3.4. A special case: clauses referring to a proposition
- 3.3.5. Adjectival phrases
- 3.3.6. Adverbial postmodification
- 3.4. Bibliographical notes
- 4 Projection of noun phrases III: binominal constructions
- Introduction
- 4.1. Binominal constructions without a preposition
- 4.2. Binominal constructions with a preposition
- 4.3. Bibliographical notes
- 5 Determiners: articles and pronouns
- Introduction
- 5.1. Articles
- 5.2. Pronouns
- 5.3. Bibliographical notes
- 6 Numerals and quantifiers
- 7 Pre-determiners
- Introduction
- 7.1. The universal quantifier al 'all' and its alternants
- 7.2. The pre-determiner heel 'all/whole'
- 7.3. A note on focus particles
- 7.4. Bibliographical notes
- 8 Syntactic uses of noun phrases
- Adjectives and Adjective Phrases
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- 2 Projection of adjective phrases I: Complementation
- 3 Projection of adjective phrases II: Modification
- 4 Projection of adjective phrases III: Comparison
- 5 Attributive use of the adjective phrase
- 6 Predicative use of the adjective phrase
- 7 The partitive genitive construction
- 8 Adverbial use of the adjective phrase
- 9 Participles and infinitives: their adjectival use
- 10 Special constructions
- Adpositions and adpositional phrases
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- Introduction
- 1.1. Characterization of the category adposition
- 1.2. A formal classification of adpositional phrases
- 1.3. A semantic classification of adpositional phrases
- 1.3.1. Spatial adpositions
- 1.3.2. Temporal adpositions
- 1.3.3. Non-spatial/temporal prepositions
- 1.4. Borderline cases
- 1.5. Bibliographical notes
- 2 Projection of adpositional phrases: Complementation
- 3 Projection of adpositional phrases: Modification
- 4 Syntactic uses of the adpositional phrase
- 5 R-pronominalization and R-words
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- Phonology
-
- General
- Phonology
- Segment inventory
- Phonotactics
- Phonological Processes
- Assimilation
- Vowel nasalization
- Syllabic sonorants
- Final devoicing
- Fake geminates
- Vowel hiatus resolution
- Vowel reduction introduction
- Schwa deletion
- Schwa insertion
- /r/-deletion
- d-insertion
- {s/z}-insertion
- t-deletion
- Intrusive stop formation
- Breaking
- Vowel shortening
- h-deletion
- Replacement of the glide w
- Word stress
- Clitics
- Allomorphy
- Orthography of Frisian
- Morphology
- Inflection
- Word formation
- Derivation
- Prefixation
- Infixation
- Suffixation
- Nominal suffixes
- Verbal suffixes
- Adjectival suffixes
- Adverbial suffixes
- Numeral suffixes
- Interjectional suffixes
- Onomastic suffixes
- Conversion
- Compositions
- Derivation
- Syntax
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Unergative and unaccusative subjects
- Evidentiality
- To-infinitival clauses
- Predication and noun incorporation
- Ellipsis
- Imperativus-pro-Infinitivo
- Expression of irrealis
- Embedded Verb Second
- Agreement
- Negation
- Nouns & Noun Phrases
- Classification
- Complementation
- Modification
- Partitive noun constructions
- Referential partitive constructions
- Partitive measure nouns
- Numeral partitive constructions
- Partitive question constructions
- Nominalised quantifiers
- Kind partitives
- Partitive predication with prepositions
- Bare nominal attributions
- Articles and names
- Pronouns
- Quantifiers and (pre)determiners
- Interrogative pronouns
- R-pronouns
- Syntactic uses
- Adjective Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Complementation
- Modification and degree quantification
- Comparison by degree
- Comparative
- Superlative
- Equative
- Attribution
- Agreement
- Attributive adjectives vs. prenominal elements
- Complex adjectives
- Noun ellipsis
- Co-occurring adjectives
- Predication
- Partitive adjective constructions
- Adverbial use
- Participles and infinitives
- Adposition Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Complementation
- Modification
- Intransitive adpositions
- Predication
- Preposition stranding
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
-
- General
- Morphology
- Morphology
- 1 Word formation
- 1.1 Compounding
- 1.1.1 Compounds and their heads
- 1.1.2 Special types of compounds
- 1.1.2.1 Affixoids
- 1.1.2.2 Coordinative compounds
- 1.1.2.3 Synthetic compounds and complex pseudo-participles
- 1.1.2.4 Reduplicative compounds
- 1.1.2.5 Phrase-based compounds
- 1.1.2.6 Elative compounds
- 1.1.2.7 Exocentric compounds
- 1.1.2.8 Linking elements
- 1.1.2.9 Separable Complex Verbs and Particle Verbs
- 1.1.2.10 Noun Incorporation Verbs
- 1.1.2.11 Gapping
- 1.2 Derivation
- 1.3 Minor patterns of word formation
- 1.1 Compounding
- 2 Inflection
- 1 Word formation
- Morphology
- Syntax
- Adjectives and adjective phrases (APs)
- 0 Introduction to the AP
- 1 Characteristics and classification of APs
- 2 Complementation of APs
- 3 Modification and degree quantification of APs
- 4 Comparison by comparative, superlative and equative
- 5 Attribution of APs
- 6 Predication of APs
- 7 The partitive adjective construction
- 8 Adverbial use of APs
- 9 Participles and infinitives as APs
- Nouns and Noun Phrases (NPs)
- 0 Introduction to the NP
- 1 Characteristics and Classification of NPs
- 2 Complementation of NPs
- 3 Modification of NPs
- 3.1 Modification of NP by Determiners and APs
- 3.2 Modification of NP by PP
- 3.3 Modification of NP by adverbial clauses
- 3.4 Modification of NP by possessors
- 3.5 Modification of NP by relative clauses
- 3.6 Modification of NP in a cleft construction
- 3.7 Free relative clauses and selected interrogative clauses
- 4 Partitive noun constructions and constructions related to them
- 4.1 The referential partitive construction
- 4.2 The partitive construction of abstract quantity
- 4.3 The numerical partitive construction
- 4.4 The partitive interrogative construction
- 4.5 Adjectival, nominal and nominalised partitive quantifiers
- 4.6 Kind partitives
- 4.7 Partitive predication with a preposition
- 4.8 Bare nominal attribution
- 5 Articles and names
- 6 Pronouns
- 7 Quantifiers, determiners and predeterminers
- 8 Interrogative pronouns
- 9 R-pronouns and the indefinite expletive
- 10 Syntactic functions of Noun Phrases
- Adpositions and Adpositional Phrases (PPs)
- 0 Introduction to the PP
- 1 Characteristics and classification of PPs
- 2 Complementation of PPs
- 3 Modification of PPs
- 4 Bare (intransitive) adpositions
- 5 Predication of PPs
- 6 Form and distribution of adpositions with respect to staticity and construction type
- 7 Adpositional complements and adverbials
- Verbs and Verb Phrases (VPs)
- 0 Introduction to the VP in Saterland Frisian
- 1 Characteristics and classification of verbs
- 2 Unergative and unaccusative subjects and the auxiliary of the perfect
- 3 Evidentiality in relation to perception and epistemicity
- 4 Types of to-infinitival constituents
- 5 Predication
- 5.1 The auxiliary of being and its selection restrictions
- 5.2 The auxiliary of going and its selection restrictions
- 5.3 The auxiliary of continuation and its selection restrictions
- 5.4 The auxiliary of coming and its selection restrictions
- 5.5 Modal auxiliaries and their selection restrictions
- 5.6 Auxiliaries of body posture and aspect and their selection restrictions
- 5.7 Transitive verbs of predication
- 5.8 The auxiliary of doing used as a semantically empty finite auxiliary
- 5.9 Supplementive predication
- 6 The verbal paradigm, irregularity and suppletion
- 7 Verb Second and the word order in main and embedded clauses
- 8 Various aspects of clause structure
- Adjectives and adjective phrases (APs)
-
- General
- Phonology
- Afrikaans phonology
- Segment inventory
- Overview of Afrikaans vowels
- The diphthongised long vowels /e/, /ø/ and /o/
- The unrounded mid-front vowel /ɛ/
- The unrounded low-central vowel /ɑ/
- The unrounded low-central vowel /a/
- The rounded mid-high back vowel /ɔ/
- The rounded high back vowel /u/
- The rounded and unrounded high front vowels /i/ and /y/
- The unrounded and rounded central vowels /ə/ and /œ/
- The diphthongs /əi/, /œy/ and /œu/
- Overview of Afrikaans consonants
- The bilabial plosives /p/ and /b/
- The alveolar plosives /t/ and /d/
- The velar plosives /k/ and /g/
- The bilabial nasal /m/
- The alveolar nasal /n/
- The velar nasal /ŋ/
- The trill /r/
- The lateral liquid /l/
- The alveolar fricative /s/
- The velar fricative /x/
- The labiodental fricatives /f/ and /v/
- The approximants /ɦ/, /j/ and /ʋ/
- Overview of Afrikaans vowels
- Word stress
- The phonetic properties of stress
- Primary stress on monomorphemic words in Afrikaans
- Background to primary stress in monomorphemes in Afrikaans
- Overview of the Main Stress Rule of Afrikaans
- The short vowels of Afrikaans
- Long vowels in monomorphemes
- Primary stress on diphthongs in monomorphemes
- Exceptions
- Stress shifts in place names
- Stress shift towards word-final position
- Stress pattern of reduplications
- Phonological processes
- Vowel related processes
- Consonant related processes
- Homorganic glide insertion
- Phonology-morphology interface
- Phonotactics
- Morphology
- Syntax
- Afrikaans syntax
- Nouns and noun phrases
- Characteristics of the NP
- Classification of nouns
- Complementation of NPs
- Modification of NPs
- Binominal and partitive constructions
- Referential partitive constructions
- Partitive measure nouns
- Numeral partitive constructions
- Partitive question constructions
- Partitive constructions with nominalised quantifiers
- Partitive predication with prepositions
- Binominal name constructions
- Binominal genitive constructions
- Bare nominal attribution
- Articles and names
- Pronouns
- Quantifiers, determiners and predeterminers
- Syntactic uses of the noun phrase
- Adjectives and adjective phrases
- Characteristics and classification of the AP
- Complementation of APs
- Modification and Degree Quantification of APs
- Comparison by comparative, superlative and equative degree
- Attribution of APs
- Predication of APs
- The partitive adjective construction
- Adverbial use of APs
- Participles and infinitives as adjectives
- Verbs and verb phrases
- Characterisation and classification
- Argument structure
- Verb frame alternations
- Complements of non-main verbs
- Verb clusters
- Complement clauses
- Adverbial modification
- Word order in the clause: Introduction
- Word order in the clause: position of the finite Verb
- Word order in the clause: Clause-initial position
- Word order in the clause: Extraposition and right-dislocation in the postverbal field
- Word order in the middle field
- Emphatic constructions
- Adpositions and adposition phrases
The sections above have shown that noun phrases are generally used to refer to certain entities in domain D. In this section, we will discuss generic noun phrases, such as those given in example (37). In examples like these, the property denoted by the verb phrase is not predicated of any entity in domain D; the examples express a general rule that is assumed to be true in the speakerʼs conception of reality. In other words, by uttering one of the generic examples in (37), the speaker claims, roughly, that, regardless of the actual choice of domain D, all zebras are striped.
a. | De zebra | is gestreept. | |
the zebra | is striped |
b. | Een zebra | is gestreept. | |
a zebra | is striped |
c. | Zebraʼs | zijn gestreept. | |
zebras | are striped |
Note that genericity is a property not only of the noun phrase, but also of the sentence as a whole. It is therefore not surprising that generic sentences have certain distinctive properties. For instance, the examples in (37) are given in the present tense, because this seems to favor the generic interpretation. This holds especially for (37a&b): replacing the present tense in these examples by a past tense results in constructions that are preferably construed as assertions about a certain individual zebra/set of zebras, and that can only marginally be interpreted as general statements on states of affairs valid for some time interval in the past. This section, however, will mainly focus on the properties of the noun phrase, though some of the properties of the generic clause as a whole will also be discussed as we go along. Subsection I starts by discussing generic uses of noun phrases headed by count nouns. This is followed in Subsection II by a discussion of generic noun phrases headed by non-count nouns.
The examples in (37) have shown that count nouns can enter three types of generic noun phrases: if the noun is singular, the article can be either definite or indefinite, and if it is plural it is normally the indefinite null article that is used.
Broadly speaking, definite noun phrases refer to the whole class or a prototype thereof, whereas indefinite noun phrases refer to typical members of the class. The fact that definite noun phrases may refer to the whole class, whereas indefinite noun phrases cannot, is clear from the examples in (38). The examples in (38b&c) are semantically anomalous since the predicate uitgestorven'extinct' can only be predicated of a species as a whole, as in (38a), not of the individual members of a species. Similar examples that do not involve natural species are given in (39).
a. | De Dodo | is uitgestorven. | |
the Dodo | is extinct |
b. | * | Een Dodo | is uitgestorven. |
a Dodo | is extinct |
c. | *? | Dodoʼs | zijn uitgestorven. |
Dodos | are extinct |
a. | De telefoon | is uitgevonden | door Alexander Graham Bell. | |
the telephone | is invented | by Alexander Graham Bell |
b. | * | Een telefoon | is uitgevonden | door Alexander Graham Bell. |
a telephone | is invented | by Alexander Graham Bell |
c. | *? | Telefoons | zijn uitgevonden | door Alexander Graham Bell. |
telephones | are invented | by Alexander Graham Bell |
The examples in (40) show that general statements that are construed as applicable to individual members of the class rather than to the class as a whole prefer a noun phrase headed by the indefinite article een or ∅. Since the proposition in (40) only holds for cats departing (to, e.g., a foreign country) and not for the whole species, the generic reading of the definite noun phrase is excluded.
a. | # | De kat | moet | zes weken voor vertrek | ingeënt | worden. |
the cat | must | six weeks before departure | vaccinated | be |
b. | Een kat | moet | zes weken voor vertrek | ingeënt | worden. | |
a cat | must | six weeks before departure | vaccinated | be | ||
'A cat must be vaccinated six weeks before departure.' |
c. | ∅ Katten | moeten | zes weken | voor vertrek | ingeënt | worden. | |
∅ cats | must | six weeks | before departure | vaccinated | be | ||
'Cats must be vaccinated six weeks before departure.' |
The examples in (41) clearly show that definite noun phrases do not have to refer to classes. The class reading of the definite noun phrase in (41a) is of course impossible, since species do not eat; only individual members of a species do. The difference between the definite and indefinite noun phrases is now that the first refers to a prototype of the class whereas the indefinite noun phrases refer to typical members of the class. This can be made clear by means of the interpretation of adverbs like meestal'generally'. In (41a), this adverb can only be interpreted as an adverb of frequency: “It is generally the case that the crocodile eats once a week (but not while guarding its eggs)”. This reading is also available for the examples in (41b&c), but in addition the adverb may quantify over the noun phrase subject resulting in the reading “Most crocodiles eat only once a week (but there are some crocodiles that eat more often)”.
a. | De krokodil | eet | meestal | maar | één keer | per week. | |
the crocodile | eats | generally | only | once | a week | ||
'Most of the time, the crocodile eats only once a week.' |
b. | Een krokodil | eet | meestal | maar | één keer | per week. | |
a crocodile | eats | generally | only | once | a week | ||
'Most of the time, a crocodile eats only once a week.' | |||||||
'Most crocodiles eat only once a week.' |
c. | Krokodillen | eten | meestal | maar | één keer | per week. | |
crocodiles | eat | generally | only | once | a week | ||
'Most of the time, crocodiles eat only once a week.' | |||||||
'Most crocodiles eat only once a week.' |
This difference becomes even clearer if the verb phrase denotes an individual-level predicate like intelligent zijn'to be intelligent', that is, a predicate that denotes a more or less permanent property of its logical subject. An example such as (42a) is considered distinctly odd by most speakers, since it expresses that most of the time the rat is intelligent, that is, it forces stage-level interpretation on the adjective intelligent. The examples in (42b&c), on the other hand, sound perfectly natural under the reading “most of”.
a. | % | De rat | is meestal | erg intelligent. |
the rat | is generally | very intelligent |
b. | Een rat | is meestal | erg intelligent. | |
a rat | is generally | very intelligent | ||
'Most rats are very intelligent.' |
c. | Ratten | zijn | meestal | erg intelligent. | |
rats | are | generally | very intelligent | ||
'Most rats are very intelligent.' |
The crucial difference between the (a)- and the (b/c)-examples in (41) and (42) is that there is only one prototype, whereas there are many typical members of a certain class: as a result only the latter can be quantified. Now that we have discussed some differences between definite and indefinite generic noun phrases, we will continue by discussing the properties of these noun phrases in more detail.
This section discusses the generic use of definite noun phrases. Since the noun phrase is normally singular, our discussion starts in Subsection 1 by considering such cases. This is followed in Subsections 2 and 3 by a discussion of whether plural definite noun phrases can also be used generically. The discussion is concluded in Subsection 4 by giving some examples of definite generic noun phrases embedded within some other noun phrase.
Generic interpretations of definite noun phrases are not encoded in some part of the noun phrase itself, but depend on the semantic content of the construction in which they occur. An example such as (43a) does not trigger a generic interpretation, since it is highly improbable that a stage-level property like being in a cage is a property of (the prototype of) the set of entities denoted by a noun like zebra'zebra'. Therefore, this sentence must be interpreted as a proposition involving a specific entity in domain D. An example such as (43a'), which involves the individual-level predicate of “being striped”, on the other hand, can be seen as a general statement about (the prototype of) this set of entities. The noun phrase de zebra can therefore be given both a generic and a referential interpretation. Note, however, that an example such as (43a') is only ambiguous on paper. Leaving contrastive accent aside, the two interpretations are distinguished by accent: on the referential reading of the noun phrase, main accent is given to the adjective gestreept; on the generic reading, on the other hand, main accent falls on the noun phrase (the noun zebra in this case). A similar difference can be observed in (43b&b').
a. | De zebra | zit | in een kooi. | specific | |
the zebra | sits | in a cage |
a'. | De zebra | is gestreept. | generic | |
the zebra | is striped |
b. | De vrouw | loopt | op straat. | specific | |
the woman | walks | in the.street |
b'. | De vrouw | is zachtmoedig van aard. | generic | |
the woman | is mild in nature |
The discussion above does not imply that the generic interpretation of definite noun phrases is completely determined by context. This becomes clear when we consider some more examples. All primeless examples in (44) would be conceivable as generic statements, which is clear from the fact that the primed examples, which involve indefinite noun phrases, actually do have the intended meanings. Nevertheless, these examples strongly favor a regular referential meaning, that is, are preferably construed as an assertion about a certain entity in domain D.
a. | # | Het meisje | is intelligent. |
the girl | is intelligent |
a'. | Meisjes | zijn | intelligent. | |
girls | are | intelligent |
b. | # | Het boek | is duur. |
the book | is expensive |
b'. | Boeken | zijn | duur. | |
books | are | expensive |
c. | # | De braadpan | is zwaar. |
the frying pan | is heavy |
c'. | Braadpannen | zijn | zwaar. | |
frying pans | are | heavy |
The reason for the impossibility of the intended generic readings of the primeless examples is not entirely clear. It might be the case that we are simply not inclined to picture a prototypical member of the sets denoted by the nouns in (44). Whereas the noun vrouw'woman' or zebra easily evokes a prototype, nouns like meisje'girl', boek'book' or braadpan'frying pan' do not. Perhaps this suggestion can be supported by the fact that a prototypical reading can be evoked provided that the context provides sufficient clues that such a reading is intended. This is clear from the fact that the examples in (45) do allow a generic reading, because the syntactic context makes it probable that two prototypes are compared: in (45a) the comparison involves a prototypical girl of a certain age and a prototypical boy of the same age, and in (45b) a prototypical girl from the polder and a prototypical girl from the city. It seems, however, that even in these cases the use of an indefinite noun phrase, as in the primed examples, is much preferred by most speakers.
a. | Het meisje | is op die leeftijd | volwassener | dan de jongen. | |
the girl | is at that age | more mature | than the boy |
a'. | Meisjes | zijn | op die leeftijd | volwassener | dan jongens. | |
girls | are | at that age | more mature | than boys |
b. | Het meisje uit de polder | is volwassener | dan het meisje uit de stad. | |
the girl from the polder | is more mature | than the girl from the city |
b'. | Meisjes uit de polder | zijn | volwassener | dan meisjes uit de stad. | |
girls from the polder | are | more mature | than girls from the city |
The generic interpretation of the noun phrases in the primeless examples in (45) is clearly facilitated by the use of the modifiers: in (45a) the use of the adverbial phrase op die leeftijd'at that age' and in (45b) by the attributively used PP uit de polder/stad'from the polder/city'. That attributive modifiers make the generic reading more readily available is also clear from the examples in (46). Perhaps the use of the attributive modifier gebonden makes a prototypical reading more readily available due to the fact that it divides the superset of books into two subsets, so that we can compare the prototypical members of these subsets: the prototypical member of the set of bound books is unaffordable, in contrast to the prototypical member of the set of paperbacks or pocket books.
a. | *? | Het boek | is tegenwoordig | onbetaalbaar. |
the book | is nowadays | unaffordable |
b. | Het gebonden boek | is tegenwoordig | onbetaalbaar. | |
the bound book | is nowadays | unaffordable |
This probably also accounts for the fact that classes that are relatively high in the speakerʼs taxonomy are normally not preceded by a definite article in generic sentences. Examples such as (47a) contrast sharply with examples such as (43b): the fact that mammals are higher in the taxonomy than zebras apparently makes it easier for the speaker to picture a prototypical zebra than a prototypical mammal. Reference to a typical member is easier, and hence the use of an indefinite article, as in (47b&c), is preferred.
a. | % | Het zoogdier | is warmbloedig. |
the mammal | is warm.blooded |
b. | Een zoogdier | is warmbloedig. | |
a mammal | is warm.blooded |
c. | Zoogdieren | zijn warmbloedig. | |
mammals | are warm.blooded |
From the discussion above, we may perhaps conclude that the ambiguity between the regular referential reading and the generic reading of a singular definite noun phrase is related to the question as to whether the language user is able to interpret the noun phrase as referring to a prototype of a certain set of entities (where many non-linguistic aspects may play a role).
Let us now consider whether plural definite noun phrases are possible in generic statements as well. Examples like (48a&a') can only be interpreted as statements about a contextually determined group of zebras/women. An example such as (48b) seems to fare better as a generic statement but this is due to the fact that the NP grote kat'big cat' may be used as the name of the superset containing the subsets of cats denoted by the nouns leeuw'lion', tijger'tiger', etc. In other words, the noun phrase de grote katten does not refer to one, but to several species of animals, hence its plural form.
a. | # | De zebraʼs | zijn | gestreept. |
the zebras | are | striped |
a'. | # | De vrouwen | zijn | zachtmoedig | van aard. |
the women | are | mild | in nature |
b. | De grote katten | zijn | gevaarlijke roofdieren. | |
the big cats | are | dangerous predators |
This seems to lead to the conclusion that plural definite noun phrases cannot be used as generic noun phrases unless the noun phrase denotes a set of entities that can be further divided into several conventionally distinguished subclasses/species. This conclusion seems to be more or less correct, but it turns out that we have to make at least one exception. Consider example (49), taken from Geerts (1984), which involves the same string of words as (48a), but which seems to be perfectly fine on a generic reading. The crucial ingredient of (49) that makes the definite determiner felicitous is the presence of the restrictive modifier alleen'only'; as soon as alleen is deleted, the output becomes bad on a generic reading. The restrictive modifier alleen is apparently able to license the use of the definite article due to the fact that it evokes a reading in which the set denoted by zebra is construed as a proper subset of a larger set, viz., the set denoted by wilde paarden'wild horses'.
(Er zijn vele soorten wilde paarden, maar) | alleen | de zebraʼs | zijn | gestreept. | ||
there are many kinds of wild horses but | only | the zebras | are | striped |
A similar effect of restrictive modifiers on the legitimacy of a definite determiner in generic plural noun phrases can be detected in the pair in (50), adapted from De Hoop, Vanden Wyngaerd & Zwart (1990: 100ff.). The semantic effect of the addition of the PP-modifier met witte voetjes is the creation of a subset of domestic cats with specific bodily features (viz., the possession of white paws); as a result, the definite determiner can now felicitously be used to pick out the intended subset.
a. | # | De katten | brengen | geluk. |
the cats | bring | luck |
b. | (Katten | hebben | een slechte reputatie, | maar) | de katten met witte voetjes | brengen | geluk. | |
cats | have | a bad reputation | but | the cats with white paws | bring | luck |
The minimal pair in (51) furthermore show that it is only the subset that can occur with the definite article; in (51a) the noun phrase de katten refers to a superset which includes the subset referred to by de katten met witte voetjes and the use of the definite article gives rise to a degraded result, whereas in (51b) the noun phrase de zwarte katten refers to a subset that is contrasted with another subset referred to by de katten met witte voetjes and the use of the definite article is allowed.
a. | *? | De katten | hebben | een slechte reputatie, | maar | de katten met witte voetjes | brengen | geluk. |
the cats | have | a bad reputation | but | the cats with white paws | bring | luck |
b. | De zwarte katten | hebben | een slechte reputatie, | maar | de katten | met witte voetjes | brengen | geluk. | |
the black cats | have | a bad reputation | but | the cats | with white paws | bring | luck |
From this we may conclude that the use of the definite article is not related to the fact that the noun phrases in question have a generic reading, but to the fact that these noun phrases are linked to some explicitly mentioned or tacitly assumed superset in domain D. This use of the definite article is therefore reminiscent of the use of the definite article in noun phrases that refer to entities that are not part of domain D but can be inferred from the linguistic or non-linguistic context of the discourse; see the discussion of examples (21) to (25) in Section 5.1.1.2.
Although the discussion in Subsection 2 has shown that plural definite noun phrases normally cannot be used generically, an exception must be made for nationality nouns like Nederlander'Dutchman' or nouns that refer to members of certain societal groups or organizations like kapitalist'capitalist'. With nouns of these types generic statements can therefore often be expressed in four different ways, as illustrated in (52) and (53).
a. | De Nederlander | is onverdraagzaam. | |
the Dutchman | is intolerant |
b. | De Nederlanders | zijn | onverdraagzaam. | |
the Dutchmen | are | intolerant |
c. | Een Nederlander | is onverdraagzaam. | |
a Dutchman | is intolerant |
d. | Nederlanders | zijn | onverdraagzaam. | |
Dutchmen | are | intolerant |
a. | De kapitalist | denkt | alleen | aan zijn eigen belangen. | |
the capitalist | thinks | only | of his own interests |
b. | De kapitalisten | denken | alleen | aan hun eigen belangen. | |
the capitalists | think | only | of their own interests |
c. | Een kapitalist | denkt | alleen | aan zijn eigen belangen. | |
a capitalist | thinks | only | of his own interests |
d. | Kapitalisten | denken | alleen | aan hun eigen belangen. | |
capitalists | think | only | of their own interests |
So far, we have only discussed generic definite noun phrases in clauses. As is shown in (54a), definite noun phrases can also obtain a generic reading when embedded in a larger noun phrase. The difference between (54a) and (54b) suggests that in this case the context also determines whether a generic reading is possible or not.
a. | [de rechten | van | [de vrouw]] | ||
the rights | of | the woman | |||
Available reading: 'the womanʼs rights' | specific | ||||
Available reading: 'womenʼs rights' | generic |
b. | [de vrienden | van | [de vrouw]] | ||
the friends | of | the woman | |||
Available reading: 'the womanʼs friends' | specific | ||||
Impossible reading: 'womenʼs friends' | generic |
Note that example (54a) is genuinely ambiguous only on paper; if pronounced in a neutral context, the generic reading will give rise to main stress on vrouw, while the specific reading assigns main prosodic prominence to rechten. This is shown in (55), where the verbal predicate blocks a generic reading of de vrouw in (55a), but strongly favors it in (55b).
a. | De rechten van de vrouw | werden | haar | allemaal | ontnomen. | |
the rights of the woman | were | her | all | taken.away | ||
'The rightrs of the woman (e.g. Marie) were all taken away from her.' |
b. | De rechten van de vrouw | worden | nog niet | universeel | erkend. | |
the rights of the woman | are | yet not | universally | recognized | ||
'Womenʼs rights arenʼt yet universally recognized.' |
The contrast between the examples in (54a) and (56) shows again that definite plural noun phrases are normally not assigned a generic reading.
[de rechten | van | [de vrouwen]] | |||
the rights | of | the women | |||
Available reading: 'the womenʼs rights' | specific | ||||
Impossible reading: 'womenʼs rights' | generic |
The examples in (37b&c) have shown that indefinite noun phrases can also be used generically. They differ from definite noun phrases in that they do not refer to a prototypical member of the set denoted by the noun. If the indefinite noun phrase is singular it refers to a typical member, and if it is plural it refers to typical members of the set denoted by the noun. In a sense, indefinite generic noun phrases “quantify” over the individuals in the set denoted by the noun; they express a categorical statement of the type “all N ...”. This is clear from the fact that these noun phrases can be modified by adverbial phrases like in het algemeen'in general', meestal'generally' or zelden'rarely', which may modify their “universal” interpretation. This was discussed already on the basis of the examples in (41) and (42). Here, we repeat examples (42b&c) as (57), which must be given the interpretation “most rats are intelligent”.
a. | Een rat | is | meestal | erg intelligent. | |
a rat | is | generally | very intelligent |
b. | Ratten | zijn | meestal | erg intelligent. | |
rats | are | generally | very intelligent |
We start in Subsection 1 by discussing some differences between generic and non-generic indefinite noun phrases. This is followed in Subsection 2 by a discussion of the differences between singular and plural generic indefinite noun phrases.
Generic indefinite noun phrases differ in syntactic behavior from the non-generic ones. Consider the examples in (58). Non-generic indefinite DPs headed by an indefinite article do not occur in the regular subject position, whereas generic indefinite noun phrases introduced by the article een/∅ must occur in this position, which is clear from the fact that they cannot enter the expletive construction discussed in Section 8.1.4; the noun phrases in (58a&a') receive a non-generic interpretation, whereas those in (58b&b') receive a generic interpretation.
a. | Er | zwemt | een vis | in het water. | non-generic | |
there | swims | a fishsg | in the water |
a'. | Er | zwemmen | vissen | in het water. | non-generic | |
there | swim | fishpl | in the water |
b. | Een vis | zwemt | in het water. | generic | |
a fishsg | swims | in the water |
b'. | Vissen | zwemmen | in het water. | generic | |
fishpl | swim | in the water |
It may be, however, that an exception must be made for generic statements of the type in (59). These examples are generic but not in the same sense as the examples discussed earlier: they do not involve a categorical statement about the members of the set denoted by the NP goed mes'good knife', but a generic statement about the activity denoted by the noun phrase dit soort werk'this kind of work'; when one does this (kind of) work, a good knife is/good knives are indispensable. Therefore, if we want to categorize the subject noun phrases in (59) as non-generic, we should rephrase our earlier findings a bit: it is only in generic clauses that indefinite noun phrases introduced by een/∅ can occupy the regular subject position. Since, to our knowledge, examples such as (59) have not been discussed in the literature, we will not address them any further.
a. | Een goed mes | is onmisbaar | voor | dit | (soort) | werk. | |
a good knife | is indispensable | for | this | kind.of | work |
b. | Goede messen | zijn onmisbaar | voor | dit | (soort) | werk. | |
good knives | are indispensable | for | this | kind.of | work |
So far, we have not discussed the difference between the singular and plural generic indefinite noun phrases. Although at first sight it seems difficult to pinpoint a difference in meaning, it is clear that they are not synonymous. This becomes evident when we consider the implication relations that hold between singular and plural examples, as in the primeless and primed examples in (60).
a. | Een zebra | is gestreept. ⇒ | |
a zebra | is striped |
a'. | Zebraʼs zijn gestreept. | |
zebras are striped |
b. | Musicals zijn populair. ⇏ | |
musicals are popular |
b'. | Een musical is populair. | |
a musical is popular |
It seems that implication relations like (60a) are always valid. The inverse implication relation in (60b), on the other hand, does not seem to hold. This suggests that generic sentences with an indefinite singular noun phrase express that the typical members of the class are in some sense inherently endowed with or defined by the property denoted by the predicate. Generic sentences with an indefinite plural noun phrases, on the other hand, seem to ascribe a more incidental or transitory property to the class: musicals may be popular today, but there is no guarantee that this will also be the case in the future. That something like this is indeed the case is clear from the fact that using an adverbial phrase like tegenwoordig'nowadays' is possible in (61a) but not in (61b).
a. | Musicals zijn | tegenwoordig | populair. | |
musicals are | nowadays | popular |
b. | * | Een musical | is tegenwoordig | populair. |
a musical | is nowadays | popular |
There are also differences concerning the syntactic environments in which singular and plural generic indefinite noun phrases can occur. Above, we have only discussed examples in which the generic noun phrase acts as the subject of a clause. When we widen our discussion to other syntactic functions, it seems that generic singular noun phrases have a more limited distribution than the plural ones. The primeless, singular examples in (62) must be construed specifically: Jan is studying or fond of a certain zebra. The primed, plural examples, on the other hand, seem to readily allow a generic interpretation of the indefinite noun phrase. The doubly-primed examples are added for completeness’ sake, in order to show that generic definite noun phrases may also be used in syntactic functions other than subject.
a. | # | Jan bestudeert | een zebra. |
Jan studies | a zebra |
b. | # | Jan is dol | op een zebra. |
Jan is fond | of a zebra |
a'. | Jan bestudeert | zebraʼs. | |
Jan studies | zebras |
b'. | Jan is dol | op zebraʼs. | |
Jan is fond | of zebras |
a''. | Jan bestudeert | de zebra. | |
Jan studies | the zebra |
b''. | Jan is dol | op de zebra. | |
Jan is fond | of the zebra |
It should be noted, however, that we cannot conclude from these examples that generic singular indefinite noun phrases can only occur as the subject of the clause. This is clear from the examples in (63). In these examples the indefinite noun phrases are not the subject of the clause, but still the sentences can be interpreted generically (which reading is facilitated if a modifier like meestal is added to the sentence). The main difference between (62) and (63) is that the examples in the latter contain a complementive that is predicated of the indefinite noun phrase. From this, we may conclude that a generic singular indefinite noun phrase can only occur if it is the logical subject of some predicate, whereas generic plural noun phrases are freer in distribution.
a. | Ik vind | een zebra | (meestal) | erg interessant. | |
I consider | a zebra | generally | very interesting |
b. | Ik vind | zebraʼs | (meestal) | erg interessant. | |
I consider | zebras | generally | very interesting |
To conclude this subsection, we give the primeless examples in (64) to show that generic indefinite noun phrases can also be embedded in a larger noun phrase. The meaning of these examples is something like “all zebras have a biotope that consists of ...”. As is shown in the primed examples, the larger noun phrase containing a singular indefinite noun phrase also has a more restricted distribution than the one containing a plural indefinite noun phrase; (64a') is at least preferably construed as involving the biotope of a certain zebra.
a. | Het biotoop | van een zebra | bestaat uit ... | |
the biotope | of a zebra | consists of |
a'. | # | Jan bestudeert | het biotoop van een zebra. |
Jan studies | the biotope of a zebra |
b. | Het biotoop | van zebraʼs | bestaat uit ... | |
the biotope | of zebras | consists of |
b'. | Jan bestudeert | het biotoop van zebraʼs. | |
Jan studies | the biotope of zebras |
Non-count nouns normally cannot be preceded by the indefinite article een. They are either preceded by a definite article or by the indefinite null article. Table 2 gives some examples of several subtypes. Subsection A starts by showing that these non-count nouns also allow a generic reading, and Subsection B continues with a discussion of some general restrictions on the distribution of generic noun phrases headed by a non-count noun.
definite | indefinite | ||
substance noun | de wijn‘the wine’ het fruit‘the fruit’ | ∅ wijn‘wine’ ∅ fruit‘fruit’ | |
abstract noun | non-deverbal | de armoede‘the poverty’ het verdriet‘the sadness’ | ∅ armoede‘poverty’ ∅ verdriet‘sadness’ |
deverbal | het roken‘the smoking’ het sigaren roken ‘the smoking of cigars’ het roken van deze sigaar ‘the smoking of this cigar’ | ∅ roken‘smoking’ ∅ sigarenroken ‘smoking of cigars’ |
This subsection discusses the generic and non-generic uses of substance nouns, which is followed by a discussion of non-deverbal and verbal abstract non-count nouns.
When a definite article combines with a substance noun like wijn'wine' or fruit'fruit', in many cases a specific interpretation for the resulting noun phrase ensues; the definite noun phrase refers to a contextually determined quantity of the substance in question. It is not impossible, however, to find substance nouns with a definite determiner that receive a generic interpretation; example (65) gives some instances of both uses.
a. | De wijn/Het fruit | is lekker. | specific | |
the wine/the fruit | is nice |
a'. | De wijn/Het fruit | is duur | dit jaar. | generic | |
the wine/the fruit | is expensive | this year |
b. | [De smaak van [de wijn/het fruit]] | is redelijk goed. | specific | |
the taste of the wine/the fruit | is reasonably good |
b'. | [De prijs van [de wijn/het fruit]] | is hoog dit jaar. | generic | |
the price of the wine/the fruit | is high this year |
The singular indefinite article een normally cannot be combined with non-count nouns. The examples in (66) show, however, that the indefinite null article ∅ can be used. If the resulting noun phrase functions as subject, its interpretation depends on its position in the clause: if the noun phrase occupies the regular subject position, as in (66a), it must be interpreted generically; if the noun phrase enters the expletive construction, as in (66a'), it is always interpreted as a non-generic, indefinite noun phrase. In other functions, the interpretation of the noun phrase depends on the denotation of the verb phrase, as can be seen by comparing the two (b)-examples.
a. | [∅ Wijn] | is lekker. | generic | |
∅ wine | is nice |
a'. | Er | ligt | nog wijn | in de kelder. | non-generic | |
there | lies | still wine | in the cellar |
b. | Jan | houdt van | wijn. | generic | |
Jan | likes | wine |
b'. | Jan | heeft | wijn | gekocht. | non-generic | |
Jan | has | wine | bought |
If the indefinite noun phrase is embedded within a larger noun phrase, it is also the context that determines whether a generic reading is available. The (a)-examples in (67) show this for substance nouns embedded in a subject, and the (b)-examples for substance nouns embedded in a direct object.
a. | [Het glas | met [∅ | wijn]] | viel | om. | non-generic | |
the glass | with | wine | fell | over | |||
'The glass containing wine tumbled.' |
a'. | [De prijs van [∅ | wijn]] | is hoog | dit jaar. | generic | |
the price of | wine | is high | this year | |||
'Wine is expensive this year.' |
b. | Jan heeft | nog | [een vat | met [∅ | goede wijn]]. | non-generic | |
Jan has | still | a barrel | with | good wine |
b'. | Jan beschreef | [de smaak van [∅ | goede wijn]]. | generic | |
Jan described | the taste of | good wine |
Non-deverbal abstract non-count nouns can likewise be construed with the definite articles de and het without necessarily receiving a specific interpretation. Again, the context determines the distribution of specific and generic readings.
a. | De armoede/Het verdriet | is ondraaglijk. | specific | |
the poverty/the sadness | is unbearable |
a'. | De armoede/Het verdriet | moet | bestreden | worden. | generic | |
the poverty/the sadness | must | eradicated | be |
b. | [de ondraaglijkheid | van | [de armoede/het verdriet]] | specific/generic | |
the unbearableness | of | the poverty/the sadness |
b'. | [De beperking | van | [de armoede/het verdriet]] | heeft | prioriteit. | generic | |
the reduction | of | the poverty/the sadness | has | priority |
Abstract non-count nouns in argument positions normally cannot be combined with the indefinite article een without triggering a special, exclamative interpretation; cf. Section 5.1.4.2. However, the addition of a restrictive modifier may license it: Er heerst daar een *(ondraaglijke) armoede'there is an unbearable poverty there'; Hij heeft een *(onzegbaar) verdriet'He has an ineffable sadness'. However, as far as we can tell, such indefinite noun phrases are not readily possible in generic contexts; the examples in (69), at least, are somewhat odd.
a. | ? | Een ondraaglijk verdriet | is moeilijk | te bestrijden. |
an unbearable sadness | is hard | to eradicate |
b'. | ? | Een onzegbaar verdriet | kan | tot | zelfmoord | leiden. |
an ineffable sadness | can | to | suicide | lead |
Abstract non-count nouns occurring with the null article, on the other hand, are possible in generic contexts. As with the substance nouns, this is the normal interpretation when an indefinite noun phrase occupies the regular subject position. In order to obtain a non-generic reading, an indefinite subject must occur in the expletive construction. If the indefinite noun phrase has some other syntactic function in the sentence, the context determines whether a generic interpretation is possible or not. If the indefinite noun phrase is embedded in a larger noun phrase the generic reading of the indefinite noun phrase is the most prominent one.
a. | Er | wordt | hier | nog steeds [∅ | armoede] | geleden. | non-generic | |
there | is | here | still | poverty | suffered |
a'. | [∅ | Armoede] | is onduldbaar | in een rijk land | als Nederland. | generic | |
poverty | is intolerable | in a rich land | like the.Netherlands |
b. | Sommige mensen | lijden | hier | nog steeds [∅ | armoede]. | non-generic | |
some people | suffer | here | still | poverty |
b'. | Deze regering | mag [∅ | armoede] | niet | accepteren. | generic | |
this government | may | poverty | not | accept |
c. | [de schande van [∅ | armoede]] | generic | |
the disgrace of | poverty |
Here we will restrict our discussion to infinitival nominals; cf. Sections 1.3.1.2 and 2.2.3.2. We start with bare-inf nominalizations (without an article). Since (71a) is derived from the intransitive, habitual verb roken'to smoke', it is not surprising that nominalizations like these are generally generic; cf. Jan rookt'Jan smokes, that is, Jan is a smoker'. The same thing holds for (71b) due to the fact that the nominalized phrase contains a bare plural noun, which seems generically construed in this example; cf. Jan rookt sigaren'Jan smokes cigars, that is, Jan is a smoker of cigars'.
a. | Roken | is slecht | voor je gezondheid. | |
smoking | is bad | for oneʼs health |
b. | Sigaren roken | is | slecht | voor je gezondheid. | |
cigar smoking | is | bad | for oneʼs health |
Det-inf nominalizations (preceded by the neuter article het) can also inherit the arguments of the verb. The examples in (72) show that if the arguments precede the infinitive, they must be realized as indefinite plural noun phrases, just as in the case of the bare-inf nominalizations, but in this case the nominalizations clearly refer to specific “smoking” events, so we may safely claim that we are dealing with non-generic uses of these noun phrases.
a. | In deze zaal | irriteert | het roken | me altijd. | |
in this room | annoys | the smoking | me always | ||
'In this room I always get annoyed by the smoking.' |
b. | In deze zaal | irriteert | het sigaren | roken | me altijd. | |
in this room | annoys | the cigars | smoking | me always |
The examples in (73) further show that when we place these noun phrases in contexts that favor a generic interpretation, the result is marginal at best.
a. | ?? | Het | roken | is slecht | voor je gezondheid. |
the | smoking | is bad | for oneʼs health |
b. | ?? | Het | sigaren roken | is slecht | voor je gezondheid. |
the | cigars smoking | is bad | for oneʼs health |
If the argument follows the infinitive, there are no restrictions on its realization; it can be plural or singular, and it can be indefinite or definite. The (a)-examples in (74) show that the choice between the latter two options affects the interpretation of the noun phrase as a whole: if the argument is a bare plural noun phrase, the generic reading of the nominalized phrase is clearly favored; if the argument is definite, on the other hand, a generic reading seems to be blocked. The (b)-examples show that as a result of this, use of a definite argument is excluded in contexts that favor a generic interpretation.
a. | Het roken | van sigaren | irriteert | me. | generic | |
the smoking | of cigars | annoys | me |
a'. | (?) | Het roken | van de sigaar/sigaren | irriteert | me. | specific |
the smoking | of the cigar/cigars | annoys | me |
b. | Het roken van sigaren | is slecht | voor je gezondheid. | generic | |
the smoking of cigars | is bad | for oneʼs health |
b'. | *? | Het roken van de sigaar/sigaren | is slecht | voor je gezondheid. |
the smoking of the cigar/cigars | is bad | for oneʼs health |
Note, however, that definite noun phrases introduced by a demonstrative are possible in contexts like (74b&b'): if the noun phrase is singular, a specific reading is triggered; if it is plural, or a substance noun like tabak'tobacco', both readings are available.
a. | Het roken van deze/die sigaar | is slecht | voor je gezondheid. | specific | |
the smoking of this/that cigar | is bad | for your health |
b. | Het roken van deze/die sigaren is slecht | voor je gezondheid. | specific/generic | |
the smoking of these/those cigars is bad | for your/oneʼs health |
Let us finally turn to cases that involve nominalized phrases embedded in a larger noun phrase. Given the contrast between the examples in (71) and (73), the judgment of example (76a) is surprising. This example clearly has a generic interpretation, but nevertheless the nominalized phrase must be preceded by the definite article. The judgments on the remaining examples (76b&c) are in accordance with the judgments on the examples in (74).
a. | [De bestrijding van | [het/??∅ | (sigaren) | roken]] | heeft prioriteit. | generic | |
the eradication of | the/∅ | cigars | smoking | has our priority |
b. | [het plezier | in | [het roken | van sigaren]] | generic | |
the pleasure | of | the smoking | of cigars |
c. | [het plezier | in | [het roken van de/deze sigaar]] | specific | |
the pleasure | of | the smoking of the/this cigar |
Although we have seen in Subsection A1 that definite noun phrases headed by a substance noun can be used generically, it is certainly not true that this holds in all cases. This will become clear by comparing the two examples in (77), which seem to show that the realization of the definite article is sensitive to the nature of the predicate; the definite article is possible (and perhaps even preferred) if we are dealing with a stage-level predicate like duur'expensive' in (77a), but not if we are dealing with an individual-level predicate like bestaan uit koolstof en waterstof'to consist of carbon and hydrogen' in (77b).
a. | De/?∅ | benzine | is | weer | duur | dit jaar. | |
the/∅ | petrol | is | again | expensive | this year | ||
'The petrol is again expensive this year.' |
b. | ∅/*De benzine | bestaat | uit koolstof en waterstof. | |
∅/the petrol | consists | of carbon and hydrogen |
An apparent counterexample to the claim that the nature of the predicate determines whether a definite article can be realized can be found in (78) which involve the individual-level predicate bestaan uit waterstof en zuurstof: the fact that using a definite article is blocked in (78a) is compatible with the proposed restriction; however, if a restrictive modifier like the PP op Mars'on Mars' is added to the generic noun phrase, as in (78b), using a definite article suddenly becomes possible.
a. | ∅/*Het water | bestaat | uit waterstof en zuurstof. | |
∅/the water | consists | of hydrogen and oxygen |
b. | Het/∅ water op Mars | bestaat | ook | uit waterstof en zuurstof. | |
the/∅ water on Mars | consists | also | of hydrogen and oxygen |
We may account for this problem by assuming that, much as in the case of (50b), the semantic effect of the addition of the modifier in (78b) is the creation of a subset/subtype of water; while water on its own defines “water” exhaustively and does not leave any subset/subtype for the definite determiner to pick out, water op Mars denotes a subtype of water found on the planet of Mars, which is not coextensive with the substance of water in general. This makes it possible for the definite determiner to be felicitously used in (78b).
From the discussion above, we conclude that, apart from those cases in which the addition of a modifier introduces a distinction between various subsets/subtypes, the definite article cannot be used if the predicate expresses an individual-level property. This conclusion seems to be supported by the examples in (79), which involve abstract non-count nouns.
a. | ∅/*De gezelligheid | kent | geen tijd. | |
∅/the coziness | knows | no time | ||
'Being sociable is always appropriate.' |
b. | ∅/*De verliefdheid | is een alles overspoelend gevoel. | |
∅/the infatuation | is an everything overflowing sensation | ||
'Infatuation is a sensation that dominates everything.' |
The infinitival nominals in (80) exhibit a pattern which is also similar yet subtly distinct from the one found in the substance noun examples in (78); cf. Hoekstra & Wehrmann (1985). Example (80a) shows again that individual-level predicates do not license the presence of a definite article: if the definite article is present, the noun phrase refers to a specific dancing event. The examples in (80b&c) show that adding a postnominal PP to the nominalization makes it possible to have a definite article due to the fact that we now dealing with various subtypes of dancing.
a. | ∅/#Het dansen | is leuk. | |
∅/the dancing | is nice |
b. | Het/∅ dansen | op blote voeten | moet | sterk | worden | ontraden. | |
the/∅ dancing | on bare feet | must | strongly | be | discouraged |
c. | Het/??∅ dansen | van sambaʼs | is een geliefde bezigheid | van Brazilianen. | |
the/∅ dancing | of sambas | is a favorite occupation | of Brazilians |
The surprising fact is that whereas the definite article is simply optional in (80b), it is preferably present in (80c). The roots of this difference with respect to the optionality of the article are as yet unclear. It is not the case, for instance, that the presence of a complement like van sambaʼs, as opposed to an adjunct like op blote voeten, makes het obligatory, which is clear from the fact that (het) werken aan je proefschrift is leuk'(the) working on your dissertation is nice' is grammatical both with and without the determiner. However, it is interesting to note that, although Dutch and English differ in that English does not allow a definite article in nominalizations where het is optional in Dutch, the two languages are perfectly on a par when it comes to the obligatory realization of the definite article preceding nominalizations with a postnominal van/of-complement: cf. the contrast between (*the) dancing on bare feet and *(the) dancing of sambas.
- 1984Algemene Nederlandse spraakkunstGroningenWolters-Noordhoff
- 1985De nominale infinitiefGLOT8257-275
- 1990Syntaxis en semantiek van de <i>van die</i>-constructieGramma1481-106