- Dutch
- Frisian
- Saterfrisian
- Afrikaans
-
- Phonology
- Segment inventory
- Phonotactics
- Phonological processes
- Phonology-morphology interface
- Word stress
- Primary stress in simplex words
- Monomorphemic words
- Diachronic aspects
- Generalizations on stress placement
- Default penultimate stress
- Lexical stress
- The closed penult restriction
- Final closed syllables
- The diphthong restriction
- Superheavy syllables (SHS)
- The three-syllable window
- Segmental restrictions
- Phonetic correlates
- Stress shifts in loanwords
- Quantity-sensitivity
- Secondary stress
- Vowel reduction in unstressed syllables
- Stress in complex words
- Primary stress in simplex words
- Accent & intonation
- Clitics
- Spelling
- Morphology
- Word formation
- Compounding
- Nominal compounds
- Verbal compounds
- Adjectival compounds
- Affixoids
- Coordinative compounds
- Synthetic compounds
- Reduplicative compounds
- Phrase-based compounds
- Elative compounds
- Exocentric compounds
- Linking elements
- Separable complex verbs (SCVs)
- Gapping of complex words
- Particle verbs
- Copulative compounds
- Derivation
- Numerals
- Derivation: inputs and input restrictions
- The meaning of affixes
- Non-native morphology
- Cohering and non-cohering affixes
- Prefixation
- Suffixation
- Nominal suffixation: person nouns
- Conversion
- Pseudo-participles
- Bound forms
- Nouns
- Nominal prefixes
- Nominal suffixes
- -aal and -eel
- -aar
- -aard
- -aat
- -air
- -aris
- -ast
- Diminutives
- -dom
- -een
- -ees
- -el (nominal)
- -elaar
- -enis
- -er (nominal)
- -erd
- -erik
- -es
- -eur
- -euse
- ge...te
- -heid
- -iaan, -aan
- -ief
- -iek
- -ier
- -ier (French)
- -ière
- -iet
- -igheid
- -ij and allomorphs
- -ijn
- -in
- -ing
- -isme
- -ist
- -iteit
- -ling
- -oir
- -oot
- -rice
- -schap
- -schap (de)
- -schap (het)
- -sel
- -st
- -ster
- -t
- -tal
- -te
- -voud
- Verbs
- Adjectives
- Adverbs
- Univerbation
- Neo-classical word formation
- Construction-dependent morphology
- Morphological productivity
- Compounding
- Inflection
- Inflection and derivation
- Allomorphy
- The interface between phonology and morphology
- Word formation
- Syntax
- Preface and acknowledgements
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
- 1 Characterization and classification
- 2 Projection of verb phrases I:Argument structure
- 3 Projection of verb phrases II:Verb frame alternations
- Introduction
- 3.1. Main types
- 3.2. Alternations involving the external argument
- 3.3. Alternations of noun phrases and PPs
- 3.3.1. Dative/PP alternations (dative shift)
- 3.3.1.1. Dative alternation with aan-phrases (recipients)
- 3.3.1.2. Dative alternation with naar-phrases (goals)
- 3.3.1.3. Dative alternation with van-phrases (sources)
- 3.3.1.4. Dative alternation with bij-phrases (possessors)
- 3.3.1.5. Dative alternation with voor-phrases (benefactives)
- 3.3.1.6. Conclusion
- 3.3.1.7. Bibliographical notes
- 3.3.2. Accusative/PP alternations
- 3.3.3. Nominative/PP alternations
- 3.3.1. Dative/PP alternations (dative shift)
- 3.4. Some apparent cases of verb frame alternation
- 3.5. Bibliographical notes
- 4 Projection of verb phrases IIIa:Selection of clauses/verb phrases
- 5 Projection of verb phrases IIIb:Argument and complementive clauses
- Introduction
- 5.1. Finite argument clauses
- 5.2. Infinitival argument clauses
- 5.3. Complementive clauses
- 6 Projection of verb phrases IIIc:Complements of non-main verbs
- 7 Projection of verb phrases IIId:Verb clusters
- 8 Projection of verb phrases IV: Adverbial modification
- 9 Word order in the clause I:General introduction
- 10 Word order in the clause II:Position of the finite verb (verb-first/second)
- 11 Word order in the clause III:Clause-initial position (wh-movement)
- Introduction
- 11.1. The formation of V1- and V2-clauses
- 11.2. Clause-initial position remains (phonetically) empty
- 11.3. Clause-initial position is filled
- 12 Word order in the clause IV:Postverbal field (extraposition)
- 13 Word order in the clause V: Middle field (scrambling)
- 14 Main-clause external elements
- Nouns and Noun Phrases
- 1 Characterization and classification
- 2 Projection of noun phrases I: complementation
- Introduction
- 2.1. General observations
- 2.2. Prepositional and nominal complements
- 2.3. Clausal complements
- 2.4. Bibliographical notes
- 3 Projection of noun phrases II: modification
- Introduction
- 3.1. Restrictive and non-restrictive modifiers
- 3.2. Premodification
- 3.3. Postmodification
- 3.3.1. Adpositional phrases
- 3.3.2. Relative clauses
- 3.3.3. Infinitival clauses
- 3.3.4. A special case: clauses referring to a proposition
- 3.3.5. Adjectival phrases
- 3.3.6. Adverbial postmodification
- 3.4. Bibliographical notes
- 4 Projection of noun phrases III: binominal constructions
- Introduction
- 4.1. Binominal constructions without a preposition
- 4.2. Binominal constructions with a preposition
- 4.3. Bibliographical notes
- 5 Determiners: articles and pronouns
- Introduction
- 5.1. Articles
- 5.2. Pronouns
- 5.3. Bibliographical notes
- 6 Numerals and quantifiers
- 7 Pre-determiners
- Introduction
- 7.1. The universal quantifier al 'all' and its alternants
- 7.2. The pre-determiner heel 'all/whole'
- 7.3. A note on focus particles
- 7.4. Bibliographical notes
- 8 Syntactic uses of noun phrases
- Adjectives and Adjective Phrases
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- 2 Projection of adjective phrases I: Complementation
- 3 Projection of adjective phrases II: Modification
- 4 Projection of adjective phrases III: Comparison
- 5 Attributive use of the adjective phrase
- 6 Predicative use of the adjective phrase
- 7 The partitive genitive construction
- 8 Adverbial use of the adjective phrase
- 9 Participles and infinitives: their adjectival use
- 10 Special constructions
- Adpositions and adpositional phrases
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- Introduction
- 1.1. Characterization of the category adposition
- 1.2. A formal classification of adpositional phrases
- 1.3. A semantic classification of adpositional phrases
- 1.3.1. Spatial adpositions
- 1.3.2. Temporal adpositions
- 1.3.3. Non-spatial/temporal prepositions
- 1.4. Borderline cases
- 1.5. Bibliographical notes
- 2 Projection of adpositional phrases: Complementation
- 3 Projection of adpositional phrases: Modification
- 4 Syntactic uses of the adpositional phrase
- 5 R-pronominalization and R-words
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- Phonology
-
- General
- Phonology
- Segment inventory
- Phonotactics
- Phonological Processes
- Assimilation
- Vowel nasalization
- Syllabic sonorants
- Final devoicing
- Fake geminates
- Vowel hiatus resolution
- Vowel reduction introduction
- Schwa deletion
- Schwa insertion
- /r/-deletion
- d-insertion
- {s/z}-insertion
- t-deletion
- Intrusive stop formation
- Breaking
- Vowel shortening
- h-deletion
- Replacement of the glide w
- Word stress
- Clitics
- Allomorphy
- Orthography of Frisian
- Morphology
- Inflection
- Word formation
- Derivation
- Prefixation
- Infixation
- Suffixation
- Nominal suffixes
- Verbal suffixes
- Adjectival suffixes
- Adverbial suffixes
- Numeral suffixes
- Interjectional suffixes
- Onomastic suffixes
- Conversion
- Compositions
- Derivation
- Syntax
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Unergative and unaccusative subjects
- Evidentiality
- To-infinitival clauses
- Predication and noun incorporation
- Ellipsis
- Imperativus-pro-Infinitivo
- Expression of irrealis
- Embedded Verb Second
- Agreement
- Negation
- Nouns & Noun Phrases
- Classification
- Complementation
- Modification
- Partitive noun constructions
- Referential partitive constructions
- Partitive measure nouns
- Numeral partitive constructions
- Partitive question constructions
- Nominalised quantifiers
- Kind partitives
- Partitive predication with prepositions
- Bare nominal attributions
- Articles and names
- Pronouns
- Quantifiers and (pre)determiners
- Interrogative pronouns
- R-pronouns
- Syntactic uses
- Adjective Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Complementation
- Modification and degree quantification
- Comparison by degree
- Comparative
- Superlative
- Equative
- Attribution
- Agreement
- Attributive adjectives vs. prenominal elements
- Complex adjectives
- Noun ellipsis
- Co-occurring adjectives
- Predication
- Partitive adjective constructions
- Adverbial use
- Participles and infinitives
- Adposition Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Complementation
- Modification
- Intransitive adpositions
- Predication
- Preposition stranding
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
-
- General
- Morphology
- Morphology
- 1 Word formation
- 1.1 Compounding
- 1.1.1 Compounds and their heads
- 1.1.2 Special types of compounds
- 1.1.2.1 Affixoids
- 1.1.2.2 Coordinative compounds
- 1.1.2.3 Synthetic compounds and complex pseudo-participles
- 1.1.2.4 Reduplicative compounds
- 1.1.2.5 Phrase-based compounds
- 1.1.2.6 Elative compounds
- 1.1.2.7 Exocentric compounds
- 1.1.2.8 Linking elements
- 1.1.2.9 Separable Complex Verbs and Particle Verbs
- 1.1.2.10 Noun Incorporation Verbs
- 1.1.2.11 Gapping
- 1.2 Derivation
- 1.3 Minor patterns of word formation
- 1.1 Compounding
- 2 Inflection
- 1 Word formation
- Morphology
- Syntax
- Adjectives and adjective phrases (APs)
- 0 Introduction to the AP
- 1 Characteristics and classification of APs
- 2 Complementation of APs
- 3 Modification and degree quantification of APs
- 4 Comparison by comparative, superlative and equative
- 5 Attribution of APs
- 6 Predication of APs
- 7 The partitive adjective construction
- 8 Adverbial use of APs
- 9 Participles and infinitives as APs
- Nouns and Noun Phrases (NPs)
- 0 Introduction to the NP
- 1 Characteristics and Classification of NPs
- 2 Complementation of NPs
- 3 Modification of NPs
- 3.1 Modification of NP by Determiners and APs
- 3.2 Modification of NP by PP
- 3.3 Modification of NP by adverbial clauses
- 3.4 Modification of NP by possessors
- 3.5 Modification of NP by relative clauses
- 3.6 Modification of NP in a cleft construction
- 3.7 Free relative clauses and selected interrogative clauses
- 4 Partitive noun constructions and constructions related to them
- 4.1 The referential partitive construction
- 4.2 The partitive construction of abstract quantity
- 4.3 The numerical partitive construction
- 4.4 The partitive interrogative construction
- 4.5 Adjectival, nominal and nominalised partitive quantifiers
- 4.6 Kind partitives
- 4.7 Partitive predication with a preposition
- 4.8 Bare nominal attribution
- 5 Articles and names
- 6 Pronouns
- 7 Quantifiers, determiners and predeterminers
- 8 Interrogative pronouns
- 9 R-pronouns and the indefinite expletive
- 10 Syntactic functions of Noun Phrases
- Adpositions and Adpositional Phrases (PPs)
- 0 Introduction to the PP
- 1 Characteristics and classification of PPs
- 2 Complementation of PPs
- 3 Modification of PPs
- 4 Bare (intransitive) adpositions
- 5 Predication of PPs
- 6 Form and distribution of adpositions with respect to staticity and construction type
- 7 Adpositional complements and adverbials
- Verbs and Verb Phrases (VPs)
- 0 Introduction to the VP in Saterland Frisian
- 1 Characteristics and classification of verbs
- 2 Unergative and unaccusative subjects and the auxiliary of the perfect
- 3 Evidentiality in relation to perception and epistemicity
- 4 Types of to-infinitival constituents
- 5 Predication
- 5.1 The auxiliary of being and its selection restrictions
- 5.2 The auxiliary of going and its selection restrictions
- 5.3 The auxiliary of continuation and its selection restrictions
- 5.4 The auxiliary of coming and its selection restrictions
- 5.5 Modal auxiliaries and their selection restrictions
- 5.6 Auxiliaries of body posture and aspect and their selection restrictions
- 5.7 Transitive verbs of predication
- 5.8 The auxiliary of doing used as a semantically empty finite auxiliary
- 5.9 Supplementive predication
- 6 The verbal paradigm, irregularity and suppletion
- 7 Verb Second and the word order in main and embedded clauses
- 8 Various aspects of clause structure
- Adjectives and adjective phrases (APs)
-
- General
- Phonology
- Afrikaans phonology
- Segment inventory
- Overview of Afrikaans vowels
- The diphthongised long vowels /e/, /ø/ and /o/
- The unrounded mid-front vowel /ɛ/
- The unrounded low-central vowel /ɑ/
- The unrounded low-central vowel /a/
- The rounded mid-high back vowel /ɔ/
- The rounded high back vowel /u/
- The rounded and unrounded high front vowels /i/ and /y/
- The unrounded and rounded central vowels /ə/ and /œ/
- The diphthongs /əi/, /œy/ and /œu/
- Overview of Afrikaans consonants
- The bilabial plosives /p/ and /b/
- The alveolar plosives /t/ and /d/
- The velar plosives /k/ and /g/
- The bilabial nasal /m/
- The alveolar nasal /n/
- The velar nasal /ŋ/
- The trill /r/
- The lateral liquid /l/
- The alveolar fricative /s/
- The velar fricative /x/
- The labiodental fricatives /f/ and /v/
- The approximants /ɦ/, /j/ and /ʋ/
- Overview of Afrikaans vowels
- Word stress
- The phonetic properties of stress
- Primary stress on monomorphemic words in Afrikaans
- Background to primary stress in monomorphemes in Afrikaans
- Overview of the Main Stress Rule of Afrikaans
- The short vowels of Afrikaans
- Long vowels in monomorphemes
- Primary stress on diphthongs in monomorphemes
- Exceptions
- Stress shifts in place names
- Stress shift towards word-final position
- Stress pattern of reduplications
- Phonological processes
- Vowel related processes
- Consonant related processes
- Homorganic glide insertion
- Phonology-morphology interface
- Phonotactics
- Morphology
- Syntax
- Afrikaans syntax
- Nouns and noun phrases
- Characteristics of the NP
- Classification of nouns
- Complementation of NPs
- Modification of NPs
- Binominal and partitive constructions
- Referential partitive constructions
- Partitive measure nouns
- Numeral partitive constructions
- Partitive question constructions
- Partitive constructions with nominalised quantifiers
- Partitive predication with prepositions
- Binominal name constructions
- Binominal genitive constructions
- Bare nominal attribution
- Articles and names
- Pronouns
- Quantifiers, determiners and predeterminers
- Syntactic uses of the noun phrase
- Adjectives and adjective phrases
- Characteristics and classification of the AP
- Complementation of APs
- Modification and Degree Quantification of APs
- Comparison by comparative, superlative and equative degree
- Attribution of APs
- Predication of APs
- The partitive adjective construction
- Adverbial use of APs
- Participles and infinitives as adjectives
- Verbs and verb phrases
- Characterisation and classification
- Argument structure
- Verb frame alternations
- Complements of non-main verbs
- Verb clusters
- Complement clauses
- Adverbial modification
- Word order in the clause: Introduction
- Word order in the clause: position of the finite Verb
- Word order in the clause: Clause-initial position
- Word order in the clause: Extraposition and right-dislocation in the postverbal field
- Word order in the middle field
- Emphatic constructions
- Adpositions and adposition phrases
The consonant /d/ is inserted between one of the consonants /n/, /l/ or /r/ and /ər/. The commonest contexts in which the sequences /nər/, /lər/, and /rər/ appear are the comparative of adjectives, subject names derived from verbs, and inhabitant names and relational adjectives derived from place names. Examples of /d/-insertion in comparatives are tin - tinder thin - thinner, fûl - fûlder intense - more intense; sharp - sharper, and fier - fierder far - farther, further. There is, however, an asymmetry here. Following /n/ and /l/, insertion is optional (and receding in extension), whereas it is obligatory (and fully productive) following /r/. This links up with a general ban on sequences in which /r/ flanks a short vowel on either side.
When preceding the sequence /ər/, the consonant /d/ is inserted, provided it is itself preceded by one of the consonants /n/, /l/ or /r/ (see Sytstra (1856:51-52), Colmjon (1863:51), Van Blom (1889:91), Sipma (1913:62), Sytstra and Hof (1925:49), Sipma (1949:20), Fokkema (1967:46)), Tiersma (1979:125-129), Tiersma (1980:12-14), Tiersma (1985:46), Tiersma (1999:31-32), Visser (1997:161-163), Hoekstra (2001:87), Popkema (2006:78)). The commonest contexts in which the above sequences appear are the comparative of adjectives, subject names derived from verbs, and inhabitant names and relational adjectives derived from place names. These are exemplified in (1):
Examples of the insertion of /d/ between /{n,l,r}/ and /ər/ | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
a. | With stems ending in /n/ | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
a.1 In the comparative of adjectives | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
brún ~ brúnder | brown ~ browner | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
grien ~ griender | green ~ greener | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
tin ~ tinder | thin ~ thinner | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
skruten ~ skrutender | timid ~ more timid | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
tefreden ~ tefredender | satisfied ~ more satisfied | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
a.2 In subject names | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
post#rinder | postman | (cf. | rinn(e) | walk, go | ) | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
priis#winder | prizewinner | (cf. | winn(e) | win | ) | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
iel#slynder | (great crested) grebe | (cf. | slin(e) | eat, feed, stuff | ) | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
a. 3 In inhabitant names and relational adjectives | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Eastereinder | inhabitant of, related to Easterein | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Westereinder | inhabitant of, related to De Westerein | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Peinder | inhabitant of, related to De Pein | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
b. | With stems ending in /l/ | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
b.1 In the comparative of adjectives | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
fol ~ folder | full ~ fuller | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
fûl ~ fûlder | intense ~ more intense; sharp ~ sharper | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
giel ~ gielder | yellow ~ yellower | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
keal ~ kealder | bald ~ balder; bare ~ barer | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
koel ~ koelder | cool ~ cooler | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
mâl ~ mâlder | silly ~ sillier; mad ~ madder | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
smel ~ smelder | narrow ~ narrower | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
stil ~ stilder | quiet ~ quieter; silent ~ more silent | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
krigel ~ krigelder | energetic ~ more energetic | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
b.2 In subject names | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
dwers#kúlder | fisherman who 'dwerskulet' | (cf. | dwerskúl(je) | fish with a 'dwerskule' | ) | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
gear#stalder | compiler | (cf. | gearstall(e) | to compile | ) | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
lulder | one who talks bullshit | (cf. | lull(e) | talk bullshit | ) | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
piano#spylder | piano player | (cf. | spyl(je) | to play | ) | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
pielder | someone who occupies himself with light, small or lowly-qualified activities | (cf. | piel(e) | occupy oneself (in a relaxed way and without compelling reasons) | ) | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
angelder | angler | (cf. | angel(je) | to angle | ) | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
dobbelder | dicer | (cf. | dobbel(je) | to dice | ) | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
gûchelder | conjurer, magician | (cf. | gûchel(je) | to conjure, do tricks | ) | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
skeakelder | switch | (cf. | skeakel(je) | to link; to connect; change gear | ) | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
sutelder | street-trader, hawker | (cf. | sutel(je) | to hawk | ) | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
b.3 In inhabitant names and relational adjectives | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Fochtelder | inhabitant of, related to De Fochtel | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Oeriselder | inhabitant of, related to the province of Oerisel | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Sondelder | inhabitant of, related to Sondel | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Terwispelder | inhabitant of, related to Terwispel | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Twizelder | inhabitant of, related to Twizel | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Skarlder | inhabitant of, related to Skarl | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
[Note: Though the spelling suggests otherwise, Skarl ends in /-rəl/ in underlying representation (see word-final sequences of a liquid and a liquid).] | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
c. | With stems ending in /r/ | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
c.1 In the comparative of adjectives | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
bizar ~ bizarder | bizarre ~ more bizarre | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
djoer ~ djoerder | expensive ~ more expensive | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
fier ~ fierder | far ~ farther, further | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
klear ~ klearder | clear ~ clearer | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
raar ~ raarder | odd, strange ~ odder, stranger | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
toar ~ toarder | barren ~ more barren; withered ~ more withered | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
nuver ~ nuverder | unusual ~ more unusual | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
suver ~ suverder | pure ~ purer | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
c.2 In subject names | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
bearder | one who makes a fuss | (cf. | bear(e) | make a fuss | ) | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
behearder | manager | (cf. | behear(e) | to manage | ) | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
bestjoerder | driver; director | (cf. | bestjoer(e) | to drive; to run | ) | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
feest#fierder | partygoer | (cf. | feestfier(e) | to celebrate | ) | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
hierder | renter; tenant | (cf. | hier(e) | to rent; to hire | ) | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
hús#warder | caretaker | (cf. | húswar(je) | look after the house | ) | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
learder | studious person | (cf. | lear(e) | to study, to learn | ) | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
oan#fierder | leader | (cf. | oanfier(e) | to lead | ) | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
ôf#stjoerder | sender | (cf. | ôfstjoer(e) | send away | ) | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
skieppe#skearder | sheepshearer | (cf. | skieppeskear(e) | shear sheep | ) | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
slange#beswarder | snake-charmer | (cf. | beswarr(e) | to conjure | ) | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
ta#hearder | listener | (cf. | tahearr(e) | to listen (to) | ) | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
tsjinst#wegerder | conscientious objector | (cf. | weger(je) | to refuse | ) | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
walfisk#farder | whaler | (cf. | farr(e) | to sail, to navigate | ) | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
heisterder | one who rushes around | (cf. | heister(je) | rush around | ) | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
kankerder | grouser, grumbler | (cf. | kanker(je) | to grouse, to grumble | ) | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
plonderder | plunderer, looter | (cf. | plonder(je) | to plunder, to loot | ) | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
sangerder | bore, nag | (cf. | sanger(je) | to nag, to wine | ) | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
c.3 In inhabitant names and relational adjectives | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Eksmoarder | inhabitant of, related to Eksmoarre | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Gaastmarder | inhabitant of, related to De Gaastmar | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Langwarder | inhabitant of, related to Langwar | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Legemarder | inhabitant of, related to Legemar | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Molkwarder | inhabitant of, related to Molkwar | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Sumarder | inhabitant of, related to Sumar | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Waskemarder | inhabitant of, related to Waskemar |
The phrasing above that /d/ is inserted when preceding the sequence ər has been chosen deliberately. It is noted in the literature − see Booij (1995:74) for the same phenomenon in Dutch − that /d/ is also inserted between a stem ending in /n/, /l/, or /r/ and the suffixes -erich, -erij, and -ernôch, as in stinderich wavery (from stinn(e) to waver), pielderich dilettanteish, amateurish (from piel(e) occupy oneself (in a relaxed way and without compelling reasons)), tsierderich quarrelsome (from tsier(e) to quarrel), strúnderij rummaging about (from strun(e) rummage about), seurderij nagging, complaining (from seur(e) to nag, to complain), djoerdernôch quite expensive; in fact too expensive (from djoer expensive), kleardernôch clearly, unequivocally (from klear clear). As with -er, insertion is optional with a stem ending in /n/ or /l/ and obligatory with an /r/-final one (see also below).
The inhabitant name and the relational adjective of the place name Frjentsjer are not Frjentsjerder, as is to be expected, but Frjentsjerter. The name Frjentsjer has developed from franaeker lord's acre, so its final /r/ is etymologically correct. More often than not, however, the name is pronounced with a final /t/, in front of which /r/ has deleted. This /t/-final form then must underly the inhabitant name and the relational adjective.
The above, however, is in need of qualification: /d/-insertion is optional with the sequences /nər/ and /lər/, but obligatory with /rər/. The derived forms in (1a,b) therefore also occur without /d/. As a matter of fact, younger generations realize ever fewer forms with /d/, to the extent that /d/-insertion has virtually stopped applying.
Place names ending in /{n,l,r}/ all have inhabitant names and relational adjectives ending in -der. As they form a set which hardly increases, if at all, it cannot be tested whether /d/-insertion still or no longer applies in the context of place names.
This does not mean, however, that the sequences /-ndər/ and /-ldər/ do not occur any more, for some words retain a once-inserted /d/, examples of which are provided in (2) (see also Tiersma (1980:13-14)):
Examples of non-alternating words in which inserted /d/ is retained | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
daalder | one guilder and a half | (cf. English | dollar | , German | Taler | ) | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
kelder | cellar | (cf. English | cellar | , German | Keller | ) | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
pylder | pillar | (cf. English | pillar | , Dutch | pilaar | ) |
These are simplex words, which do not alternate with /d/-less forms, so /d/ has been reinterpreted as part of the underlying representation.
Such an explanation, however, is not available for all words with /d/. As a first example, take bjinder and bjinner, both deriving from the verb bjinn(e) to scrub. That bjinder is still in use is likely to be due to the fact that it has developed a meaning different from that of bjinner. Whereas the former has become an instrument noun, meaning scrubbing-brush, scrubber, the latter is, and remains, a pure agent noun, meaning one who scrubs. Meaning differentiation thus may be a source of preservation. In Dutch the same holds for the relation between boender scrubbing-brush, scrubber and boener one who scrubs− deriving from the verb boen(en) to scrub−, which may have had an influence on the Frisian word pair bjinder - bjinner.
Take, as a second example, the word minder less, fewer. This is the comparative of the quantifier min little, few. The latter, however, can only be used in combination with the adverbs te too and sa so (te min too little, too few, sa min mooglik as little, as few as possible). In other contexts, a small quantity of something is indicated by phrases like net folle not much, many, in pear a few (literally: a pair), in bytsje a little bit, and the like. This means that minder does not have a transparent compositional meaning, viz. min plus comparative, which in all likelihood is the reason why it is retained as such. Again, the same may hold for Dutch minder less, fewer, the comparative of weinig little, few.
As a third example, take compounds ending in -gunder one who grants another one something (derived from the verb gunn(e) to grant): goegunder donor, supporter, kweagunder envious, jealous person, and nearinggunder one who gives you his custom.
Finally, take tsjoender magician, sorcerer, wizard. Though it seems to have a transparent relation with the verb tsjoen(e) work magic, its /d/ is retained; this also goes for the derived noun tsjoenderij magic, sorcery, witchcraft. An explanation for /d/'s retention here is not readily available.
Next to less, fewer, the word minder also means worse, in which case it is the comparative of min bad. Despite the fact that minder has a transparent compositional meaning here, its /d/ is obligatory, for the form *minner does not occur.
The word sigeuner gypsy has the variant sigeunder; word-final -er must have been reinterpreted as a suffix here.
Frisian has also borrowed words, with /d/ and all, from Dutch. Two examples are einder horizon and stander stand (of a bicycle, for instance).
The prefix aller- is attached to the superlative of adjectives, either to denote a still higher degree than the superlative does or to denote a high degree just like that. In the great majority of instances, however, the variant alder- is used, as in (it) alder#moaist most beautiful by far and alder#aardichst very, very nice. In fact, according to Sytstra and Hof (1925:49)alder- is the only form possible. It is also used in case of prefix doubling, as with it wie alder-alderferskuorrendste min waar the weather was horribly bad (also in nominal use, like do bist myn alderalderste you are the one I love most of all). Confer the variant alderbalder (< alder by alder), as in alderbaldermoaist very, very beautiful.
alder- is also the first part of the adjective alderhande, which is used in denoting a group of comparable objects, as in alderhande stuollen chairs of all sorts/kinds, all sorts/kinds of chairs, and alderlei, which by and large has the same function, as in alderlei drokte all sorts/kinds of fuss, bother. It is also found in Alderheljen All Saints'Day and Aldersielen All Soul's Day. This alder- is related to the numeral al/alle all.
Additional evidence for the difference between the sequences /nər/ and /lər/ on the one and /rər/ on the other hand, is provided by some cliticization facts. If the third person singular preterite form of a strong/irregular verb ending in /-{n/l}/ is followed by the clitic form of the third person singular masculine personal pronoun, both allomorphs of the latter, i.e. er /ər/ and der /dər/, can show up, whereas only der is allowed following an /r/-final stem (Visser (1997:162-163)). See the examples in (3):
Examples of the occurrence of the clitics 'er' and 'der' following a preterite stem ending in {/n,l,r/} | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
a. | With stems ending in /n/ | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
dat fûn er/der dêr net | that found he there not | he didn't find that there | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
b. | With stems ending in /l/ | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
dêr foel er/der dea del | there fell he dead down | there he dropped dead | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
c. | With stems ending in /r/ | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
sa fear der/*er fuort | so sailed he away | that's how he sailed away | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
sok iten sear der/*er altyd | such food cooked he always | that's the kind of food he always cooked |
Since er and der must be considered as independent variants (see cliticization), there is no /d/-insertion here. The above pattern, however, is indicative of the difference between the sequences /{n/l}ər/ and /rər/.
What is behind the insertion of /d/ between /{n/l}/ and /ər/ or, put differently, what is it that makes the sequences /nər/ and /lər/ eligible for /d/-insertion? Note first that /{n/l/r/}/, being sonorant consonants, share many features and second that they are separated by schwa here. Since the latter is an essentially featureless, minimally specified vowel (see schwa's phonological representation), the consonants in these sequences are virtually adjacent. Tiersma (1980:12) therefore considers /d/-insertion a process which is functionally equivalent to a dissimilation process in that it prevents sequences of (sonorant) consonants which are identical or very similar to one another. If the sequences /nər/ and /lər/ result from morphological processes, they are repaired by inserting the obstruent which shares the largest number of features with the sonorant consonants, namely d. The dissimilatory effect then seems to reside in the fact that /d/ is an obstruent, in strong contrast with the sonorant consonants by which it is surrounded. Sonorancy, however, cannot be the only factor here, for the sequences /mər/ and /ŋər/ are fine as such, i.e. they are not broken up by /d/ or any other obstruent.
Now, /{n,l,r}/ share more than a high degree of sonorancy. They all are coronal consonants, which suggests that their place specification might (also) play a role. Is /d/-insertion triggered then by the fact that the consonants of the sequences /nər/ and /lər/ have the same place of articulation? Note that this would explain the non-occurrence of /d/-insertion with respect to the sequences /mər/ and /ər/, which consist of the bilabial/velar nasal consonants /{m/ŋ}/ and the coronal liquid (rhotic) /r/. The same place specification of the segments in the sequences /nər/ and /lər/ causes an OCP violation, be it only a slight one, since after all the consonants are separated by schwa. However, since /d/ is a coronal consonant as well, nothing seems to be gained by its insertion. At the level of sonorancy, however, /d/-insertion does make for a better contour within these sequences, for the value -sonorant of /d/ contrasts with the +sonorant value of the surrounding consonants. Be that as it may, it seems highly unlikely that an OCP violation with respect to the place specification of adjacent consonants can be repaired by establishing a contour at the level of sonorancy.
As noted above, the younger generations realize ever fewer forms ending in /-ndər/ and /-ldər/, to the extent that /d/-insertion has virtually stopped applying. This may have to do with the status of the process, that is, its unclear constraints-and-repair-status (see the previous paragraph) may have furthered its disappearance from the grammar.
With respect to the sequence /rər/, /d/-insertion is obligatory, a fact which cannot but yield a different explanation than for the sequences /nər/ and /lər/. This will be discussed now (see also Visser (1997:161-163)). The liquid /r/ cannot flank a short monophthong on either side (see Booij (1995:43) as to Dutch, whereas it is maintained in Booij (1995:73) that this is a cross-linguistic tendency). This constraint on the occurrence of /r/ might be formalized as in (4):
The constraint expresses the fact that raar /ra:r/ odd, strange, queer, rear(e) /rɪər/ stir, rier /riər/ heifer, skroar /skroər/ tailor, clothier, roer /ruər/ rudder, treur(je) /trøər/ sorrow, mourn are possible, whereas *rar /rar/, *rir /rɪr/, *ryr /rir/, etc. are not. Words ending in /-rər/ are excluded as well (be they simplex or derived).
The interjection (wild oath) harrekrarre /harəkrarə/ ugh! is an exception.
The fact that /r/ can flank (short) rising diphthongs, as in beruor(je) /bərwor/ arrange, fix (up) and (adapted) loan words ending in -ear(je) ( /-jɛr/), like orear(je) /o:rjɛr/ orate, declaim and kastrearje /kastrjɛr/ castrate, is an indication that a rising diphthong consists of two components at the segmental level.
The /r/-constraint has repercussions for the realization of complex words and combinations of host word + clitic, and for the operation of phonological rules.
Firstly, the constraint implies that /d/-insertion is obligatory if the stem preceding /ər/ ends in /r/. This is exemplified in (1a) of /r/-deletion in complex words derived with a suffix which contains the vowel schwa and in (1c) above (see Booij (1995:73) for examples of the same phenomenon in Dutch). This implies that
- the agent noun of hier(e) /hiər/ hire, rent, lease is hierder tenant, renter, hirer (not *hierer);
- the comparative of djoer /djuər/ expensive, costly is djoerder (not *djoerer);
- only the adjective tsierderich quarrelsome and the noun tsierderij arguing, quarrelling can stand next to the verb tsier(e) /tsiər/ quarrel (and not *tsiererich and *tsiererij);
- only the inhabitant names/relational adjectives Langwarder and Sumarder can stand next to Langwar and Sumar (and not *Langwarrer and *Sumarrer).
The same holds for the cliticization case in (3c) above, repeated here as (4): if the preterite stem ends in /-r/, only the clitic allomorph der is possible:
Examples of the insertion of /d/ in the context of cliticization | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
sa fear der/*er fuort | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
sok iten sear der/*er altyd |
Secondly, both the insertion of /d/ between stem-final /r/ and /ər/ and the subsequent deletion of /r/ before inserted /d/ are obligatory processes, examples of which are also given in /r/-deletion in complex words derived with a suffix which contains the vowel schwa. This means that only the sequence /-dər/ remains, see (5):
The realization of forms ending in the sequence /-rdər/ | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
hierder | [hiədr̩] | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
djoerder | [djuədr̩] | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
tsierderich | [tsiədərəx] | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
tsierderij | [tsiədərɛj] | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
fearder | [fɪədr̩] | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
searder | [sɪədr̩] | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Langwarder | [laŋvadr̩] | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Sumarder | [səmadr̩] |
Since /d/ is inserted before more than one suffix and in phonological words consisting of a host word and a clitic, the process can be described in purely phonological terms.
As noted, /d/ is obligatorily inserted between /r/ and -er and the inserted /d/, in its turn, causes the obligatory deletion of /r/. This means that in forms like those in (5) /r/ is recoverable via /d/, a segment belonging to neither stem nor suffix.
When following a stressed and preceding an unstressed vowel, /d/ can (could) turn into /r/, which gives (gave) rise to a great deal of dialectal variation (see intervocalic /d/ and /r/). In some dialects then the verbs wurd(e) /vød/ become and hâld(e) /hɔ:d/ to hold, to keep became wurr(e) /vør/ and hâr(e) /hɔ:r/. But although the verbs biede /biəd/ to offer and siede /siəd/ to boil; to cook turned into bier(e) /biər/ and sier(e) /siər/ dialectally, the agent nouns bieder /biəd+ər/ bidder and (iten)sieder /(itən#)-siəd+ər/ who does the cooking could not possibly become *bierer and *(iten)-sierer. The impossibility of /d/-rhotacism here links up with the ill-formedness of the sequence /*rər/.
An equally revealing case is the noun ljedder /ljɛdər/ ladder; scale, which has the dialectal variant ljedde /ljɛdə/. But whereas the form *ljerrer (from ljedder) does not occur, the form ljerre (from ljedde) does.
The ending -earje of loanverbs can be realized as [ɪərjə] or [jɛrjə], according to dialect, so the pronunciation of sollisitearje apply (for) is either [sɔlisitɪərjə] or [sɔlisitjɛrjə]. In the great majority of cases, the agent nouns accompanying these verbs are realized with [ɪə] and not with [jɛ], which means that sortearder sorter− from sortearje to sort (out)− is predominantly pronounced as [sɔrtɪədr̩] (and not as [sɔrtjɛdr̩]). This preference for [ɪə] may be ascribed to the fact that /r/-deletion, which is obligatory here, fares better when following a long vocalic sequence than when following a short vowel (see /r/-deletion).
- 1889Beknopte friesche Spraakkunst voor den tegenwoordigen tijdLeeuwardenJ.W. Muller
- 1995The phonology of DutchOxfordOxford University Press
- 1995The phonology of DutchOxfordOxford University Press
- 1995The phonology of DutchOxfordOxford University Press
- 1995The phonology of DutchOxfordOxford University Press
- 1863Beknopte Friesche spraakkunst voor den tegenwoordigen tijdZijlstra, Joure
- 1967Beknopte Friese SpraakkunstFryske Akademy, Leeuwarden
- 2001Standard West FrisianMunske, Horst Haider, Århammar, Nils, Hoekstra, J.F., Vries, O., Walker, A.G.H., Wilts, O. & Faltings, V.F. (eds.)Handbuch des Friesischen / Handbook of Frisian StudiesMax Niemeyer83-98
- 2006Grammatica FriesUtrecht/ LjouwertUitgeverij Het Spectrum BV Prisma Woordenboeken en Taaluitgaven/ Fryske Akademy
- 1913Phonology and Grammar of Modern West FrisianLondon, New YorkOxford University Press
- 1949Ta it Frysk IIIR. van der Velde, Ljouwert
- 1856Klank- en Schriftleer der Friesche taal, of eerste afdeeling der Friesche SpraakkunstLeeuwarden
- 1925Nieuwe Friesche SpraakkunstLeeuwardenR. van der Velde
- 1925Nieuwe Friesche SpraakkunstLeeuwardenR. van der Velde
- 1979Aspects of the phonology of Frisian based on the language of GrouMeidielingen fan de stúdzjerjochting Frysk oan de Frije Universiteit yn Amsterdam4
- 1980The lexicon in phonological theoryBloomington, Ind.Indiana University Linguistics ClubThesis
- 1980The lexicon in phonological theoryBloomington, Ind.Indiana University Linguistics ClubThesis
- 1980The lexicon in phonological theoryBloomington, Ind.Indiana University Linguistics ClubThesis
- 1985Frisian reference grammarDordrechtForis Publications
- 1999Frisian Reference GrammarAfûk, Ljouwert
- 1997The Syllable in FrisianVrije Universiteit AmsterdamThesis
- 1997The Syllable in FrisianVrije Universiteit AmsterdamThesis
- 1997The Syllable in FrisianVrije Universiteit AmsterdamThesis