- Dutch
- Frisian
- Saterfrisian
- Afrikaans
-
- Phonology
- Segment inventory
- Phonotactics
- Phonological processes
- Phonology-morphology interface
- Word stress
- Primary stress in simplex words
- Monomorphemic words
- Diachronic aspects
- Generalizations on stress placement
- Default penultimate stress
- Lexical stress
- The closed penult restriction
- Final closed syllables
- The diphthong restriction
- Superheavy syllables (SHS)
- The three-syllable window
- Segmental restrictions
- Phonetic correlates
- Stress shifts in loanwords
- Quantity-sensitivity
- Secondary stress
- Vowel reduction in unstressed syllables
- Stress in complex words
- Primary stress in simplex words
- Accent & intonation
- Clitics
- Spelling
- Morphology
- Word formation
- Compounding
- Nominal compounds
- Verbal compounds
- Adjectival compounds
- Affixoids
- Coordinative compounds
- Synthetic compounds
- Reduplicative compounds
- Phrase-based compounds
- Elative compounds
- Exocentric compounds
- Linking elements
- Separable complex verbs (SCVs)
- Gapping of complex words
- Particle verbs
- Copulative compounds
- Derivation
- Numerals
- Derivation: inputs and input restrictions
- The meaning of affixes
- Non-native morphology
- Cohering and non-cohering affixes
- Prefixation
- Suffixation
- Nominal suffixation: person nouns
- Conversion
- Pseudo-participles
- Bound forms
- Nouns
- Nominal prefixes
- Nominal suffixes
- -aal and -eel
- -aar
- -aard
- -aat
- -air
- -aris
- -ast
- Diminutives
- -dom
- -een
- -ees
- -el (nominal)
- -elaar
- -enis
- -er (nominal)
- -erd
- -erik
- -es
- -eur
- -euse
- ge...te
- -heid
- -iaan, -aan
- -ief
- -iek
- -ier
- -ier (French)
- -ière
- -iet
- -igheid
- -ij and allomorphs
- -ijn
- -in
- -ing
- -isme
- -ist
- -iteit
- -ling
- -oir
- -oot
- -rice
- -schap
- -schap (de)
- -schap (het)
- -sel
- -st
- -ster
- -t
- -tal
- -te
- -voud
- Verbs
- Adjectives
- Adverbs
- Univerbation
- Neo-classical word formation
- Construction-dependent morphology
- Morphological productivity
- Compounding
- Inflection
- Inflection and derivation
- Allomorphy
- The interface between phonology and morphology
- Word formation
- Syntax
- Preface and acknowledgements
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
- 1 Characterization and classification
- 2 Projection of verb phrases I:Argument structure
- 3 Projection of verb phrases II:Verb frame alternations
- Introduction
- 3.1. Main types
- 3.2. Alternations involving the external argument
- 3.3. Alternations of noun phrases and PPs
- 3.3.1. Dative/PP alternations (dative shift)
- 3.3.1.1. Dative alternation with aan-phrases (recipients)
- 3.3.1.2. Dative alternation with naar-phrases (goals)
- 3.3.1.3. Dative alternation with van-phrases (sources)
- 3.3.1.4. Dative alternation with bij-phrases (possessors)
- 3.3.1.5. Dative alternation with voor-phrases (benefactives)
- 3.3.1.6. Conclusion
- 3.3.1.7. Bibliographical notes
- 3.3.2. Accusative/PP alternations
- 3.3.3. Nominative/PP alternations
- 3.3.1. Dative/PP alternations (dative shift)
- 3.4. Some apparent cases of verb frame alternation
- 3.5. Bibliographical notes
- 4 Projection of verb phrases IIIa:Selection of clauses/verb phrases
- 5 Projection of verb phrases IIIb:Argument and complementive clauses
- Introduction
- 5.1. Finite argument clauses
- 5.2. Infinitival argument clauses
- 5.3. Complementive clauses
- 6 Projection of verb phrases IIIc:Complements of non-main verbs
- 7 Projection of verb phrases IIId:Verb clusters
- 8 Projection of verb phrases IV: Adverbial modification
- 9 Word order in the clause I:General introduction
- 10 Word order in the clause II:Position of the finite verb (verb-first/second)
- 11 Word order in the clause III:Clause-initial position (wh-movement)
- Introduction
- 11.1. The formation of V1- and V2-clauses
- 11.2. Clause-initial position remains (phonetically) empty
- 11.3. Clause-initial position is filled
- 12 Word order in the clause IV:Postverbal field (extraposition)
- 13 Word order in the clause V: Middle field (scrambling)
- 14 Main-clause external elements
- Nouns and Noun Phrases
- 1 Characterization and classification
- 2 Projection of noun phrases I: complementation
- Introduction
- 2.1. General observations
- 2.2. Prepositional and nominal complements
- 2.3. Clausal complements
- 2.4. Bibliographical notes
- 3 Projection of noun phrases II: modification
- Introduction
- 3.1. Restrictive and non-restrictive modifiers
- 3.2. Premodification
- 3.3. Postmodification
- 3.3.1. Adpositional phrases
- 3.3.2. Relative clauses
- 3.3.3. Infinitival clauses
- 3.3.4. A special case: clauses referring to a proposition
- 3.3.5. Adjectival phrases
- 3.3.6. Adverbial postmodification
- 3.4. Bibliographical notes
- 4 Projection of noun phrases III: binominal constructions
- Introduction
- 4.1. Binominal constructions without a preposition
- 4.2. Binominal constructions with a preposition
- 4.3. Bibliographical notes
- 5 Determiners: articles and pronouns
- Introduction
- 5.1. Articles
- 5.2. Pronouns
- 5.3. Bibliographical notes
- 6 Numerals and quantifiers
- 7 Pre-determiners
- Introduction
- 7.1. The universal quantifier al 'all' and its alternants
- 7.2. The pre-determiner heel 'all/whole'
- 7.3. A note on focus particles
- 7.4. Bibliographical notes
- 8 Syntactic uses of noun phrases
- Adjectives and Adjective Phrases
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- 2 Projection of adjective phrases I: Complementation
- 3 Projection of adjective phrases II: Modification
- 4 Projection of adjective phrases III: Comparison
- 5 Attributive use of the adjective phrase
- 6 Predicative use of the adjective phrase
- 7 The partitive genitive construction
- 8 Adverbial use of the adjective phrase
- 9 Participles and infinitives: their adjectival use
- 10 Special constructions
- Adpositions and adpositional phrases
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- Introduction
- 1.1. Characterization of the category adposition
- 1.2. A formal classification of adpositional phrases
- 1.3. A semantic classification of adpositional phrases
- 1.3.1. Spatial adpositions
- 1.3.2. Temporal adpositions
- 1.3.3. Non-spatial/temporal prepositions
- 1.4. Borderline cases
- 1.5. Bibliographical notes
- 2 Projection of adpositional phrases: Complementation
- 3 Projection of adpositional phrases: Modification
- 4 Syntactic uses of the adpositional phrase
- 5 R-pronominalization and R-words
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- Phonology
-
- General
- Phonology
- Segment inventory
- Phonotactics
- Phonological Processes
- Assimilation
- Vowel nasalization
- Syllabic sonorants
- Final devoicing
- Fake geminates
- Vowel hiatus resolution
- Vowel reduction introduction
- Schwa deletion
- Schwa insertion
- /r/-deletion
- d-insertion
- {s/z}-insertion
- t-deletion
- Intrusive stop formation
- Breaking
- Vowel shortening
- h-deletion
- Replacement of the glide w
- Word stress
- Clitics
- Allomorphy
- Orthography of Frisian
- Morphology
- Inflection
- Word formation
- Derivation
- Prefixation
- Infixation
- Suffixation
- Nominal suffixes
- Verbal suffixes
- Adjectival suffixes
- Adverbial suffixes
- Numeral suffixes
- Interjectional suffixes
- Onomastic suffixes
- Conversion
- Compositions
- Derivation
- Syntax
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Unergative and unaccusative subjects
- Evidentiality
- To-infinitival clauses
- Predication and noun incorporation
- Ellipsis
- Imperativus-pro-Infinitivo
- Expression of irrealis
- Embedded Verb Second
- Agreement
- Negation
- Nouns & Noun Phrases
- Classification
- Complementation
- Modification
- Partitive noun constructions
- Referential partitive constructions
- Partitive measure nouns
- Numeral partitive constructions
- Partitive question constructions
- Nominalised quantifiers
- Kind partitives
- Partitive predication with prepositions
- Bare nominal attributions
- Articles and names
- Pronouns
- Quantifiers and (pre)determiners
- Interrogative pronouns
- R-pronouns
- Syntactic uses
- Adjective Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Complementation
- Modification and degree quantification
- Comparison by degree
- Comparative
- Superlative
- Equative
- Attribution
- Agreement
- Attributive adjectives vs. prenominal elements
- Complex adjectives
- Noun ellipsis
- Co-occurring adjectives
- Predication
- Partitive adjective constructions
- Adverbial use
- Participles and infinitives
- Adposition Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Complementation
- Modification
- Intransitive adpositions
- Predication
- Preposition stranding
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
-
- General
- Morphology
- Morphology
- 1 Word formation
- 1.1 Compounding
- 1.1.1 Compounds and their heads
- 1.1.2 Special types of compounds
- 1.1.2.1 Affixoids
- 1.1.2.2 Coordinative compounds
- 1.1.2.3 Synthetic compounds and complex pseudo-participles
- 1.1.2.4 Reduplicative compounds
- 1.1.2.5 Phrase-based compounds
- 1.1.2.6 Elative compounds
- 1.1.2.7 Exocentric compounds
- 1.1.2.8 Linking elements
- 1.1.2.9 Separable Complex Verbs and Particle Verbs
- 1.1.2.10 Noun Incorporation Verbs
- 1.1.2.11 Gapping
- 1.2 Derivation
- 1.3 Minor patterns of word formation
- 1.1 Compounding
- 2 Inflection
- 1 Word formation
- Morphology
- Syntax
- Adjectives and adjective phrases (APs)
- 0 Introduction to the AP
- 1 Characteristics and classification of APs
- 2 Complementation of APs
- 3 Modification and degree quantification of APs
- 4 Comparison by comparative, superlative and equative
- 5 Attribution of APs
- 6 Predication of APs
- 7 The partitive adjective construction
- 8 Adverbial use of APs
- 9 Participles and infinitives as APs
- Nouns and Noun Phrases (NPs)
- 0 Introduction to the NP
- 1 Characteristics and Classification of NPs
- 2 Complementation of NPs
- 3 Modification of NPs
- 3.1 Modification of NP by Determiners and APs
- 3.2 Modification of NP by PP
- 3.3 Modification of NP by adverbial clauses
- 3.4 Modification of NP by possessors
- 3.5 Modification of NP by relative clauses
- 3.6 Modification of NP in a cleft construction
- 3.7 Free relative clauses and selected interrogative clauses
- 4 Partitive noun constructions and constructions related to them
- 4.1 The referential partitive construction
- 4.2 The partitive construction of abstract quantity
- 4.3 The numerical partitive construction
- 4.4 The partitive interrogative construction
- 4.5 Adjectival, nominal and nominalised partitive quantifiers
- 4.6 Kind partitives
- 4.7 Partitive predication with a preposition
- 4.8 Bare nominal attribution
- 5 Articles and names
- 6 Pronouns
- 7 Quantifiers, determiners and predeterminers
- 8 Interrogative pronouns
- 9 R-pronouns and the indefinite expletive
- 10 Syntactic functions of Noun Phrases
- Adpositions and Adpositional Phrases (PPs)
- 0 Introduction to the PP
- 1 Characteristics and classification of PPs
- 2 Complementation of PPs
- 3 Modification of PPs
- 4 Bare (intransitive) adpositions
- 5 Predication of PPs
- 6 Form and distribution of adpositions with respect to staticity and construction type
- 7 Adpositional complements and adverbials
- Verbs and Verb Phrases (VPs)
- 0 Introduction to the VP in Saterland Frisian
- 1 Characteristics and classification of verbs
- 2 Unergative and unaccusative subjects and the auxiliary of the perfect
- 3 Evidentiality in relation to perception and epistemicity
- 4 Types of to-infinitival constituents
- 5 Predication
- 5.1 The auxiliary of being and its selection restrictions
- 5.2 The auxiliary of going and its selection restrictions
- 5.3 The auxiliary of continuation and its selection restrictions
- 5.4 The auxiliary of coming and its selection restrictions
- 5.5 Modal auxiliaries and their selection restrictions
- 5.6 Auxiliaries of body posture and aspect and their selection restrictions
- 5.7 Transitive verbs of predication
- 5.8 The auxiliary of doing used as a semantically empty finite auxiliary
- 5.9 Supplementive predication
- 6 The verbal paradigm, irregularity and suppletion
- 7 Verb Second and the word order in main and embedded clauses
- 8 Various aspects of clause structure
- Adjectives and adjective phrases (APs)
-
- General
- Phonology
- Afrikaans phonology
- Segment inventory
- Overview of Afrikaans vowels
- The diphthongised long vowels /e/, /ø/ and /o/
- The unrounded mid-front vowel /ɛ/
- The unrounded low-central vowel /ɑ/
- The unrounded low-central vowel /a/
- The rounded mid-high back vowel /ɔ/
- The rounded high back vowel /u/
- The rounded and unrounded high front vowels /i/ and /y/
- The unrounded and rounded central vowels /ə/ and /œ/
- The diphthongs /əi/, /œy/ and /œu/
- Overview of Afrikaans consonants
- The bilabial plosives /p/ and /b/
- The alveolar plosives /t/ and /d/
- The velar plosives /k/ and /g/
- The bilabial nasal /m/
- The alveolar nasal /n/
- The velar nasal /ŋ/
- The trill /r/
- The lateral liquid /l/
- The alveolar fricative /s/
- The velar fricative /x/
- The labiodental fricatives /f/ and /v/
- The approximants /ɦ/, /j/ and /ʋ/
- Overview of Afrikaans vowels
- Word stress
- The phonetic properties of stress
- Primary stress on monomorphemic words in Afrikaans
- Background to primary stress in monomorphemes in Afrikaans
- Overview of the Main Stress Rule of Afrikaans
- The short vowels of Afrikaans
- Long vowels in monomorphemes
- Primary stress on diphthongs in monomorphemes
- Exceptions
- Stress shifts in place names
- Stress shift towards word-final position
- Stress pattern of reduplications
- Phonological processes
- Vowel related processes
- Consonant related processes
- Homorganic glide insertion
- Phonology-morphology interface
- Phonotactics
- Morphology
- Syntax
- Afrikaans syntax
- Nouns and noun phrases
- Characteristics of the NP
- Classification of nouns
- Complementation of NPs
- Modification of NPs
- Binominal and partitive constructions
- Referential partitive constructions
- Partitive measure nouns
- Numeral partitive constructions
- Partitive question constructions
- Partitive constructions with nominalised quantifiers
- Partitive predication with prepositions
- Binominal name constructions
- Binominal genitive constructions
- Bare nominal attribution
- Articles and names
- Pronouns
- Quantifiers, determiners and predeterminers
- Syntactic uses of the noun phrase
- Adjectives and adjective phrases
- Characteristics and classification of the AP
- Complementation of APs
- Modification and Degree Quantification of APs
- Comparison by comparative, superlative and equative degree
- Attribution of APs
- Predication of APs
- The partitive adjective construction
- Adverbial use of APs
- Participles and infinitives as adjectives
- Verbs and verb phrases
- Characterisation and classification
- Argument structure
- Verb frame alternations
- Complements of non-main verbs
- Verb clusters
- Complement clauses
- Adverbial modification
- Word order in the clause: Introduction
- Word order in the clause: position of the finite Verb
- Word order in the clause: Clause-initial position
- Word order in the clause: Extraposition and right-dislocation in the postverbal field
- Word order in the middle field
- Emphatic constructions
- Adpositions and adposition phrases
The minimal pair below illustrates the sensitivity of adjectival agreement to number in the case of neuter singular NPs:
‘n | Oold | Huus. | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
a | old | house.NTR.SG | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
An old house. |
Oolde | Huze. | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
old | House.PL | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Old houses. |
The examples illustrate that the form of the adjective changes in the plural, at least for neuter nouns. Incidentally, the minimal pair also shows that the indefinite determiner is absent in the plural. In addition, gender agreement on AP shows up in the singular (only). The minimal pair below illustrates the sensitivity of adjectival agreement to gender for singular NPs:
‘n | Oold-en | Disk, | ‘n | oold-e | Ku, | ‘n | oold | Huus. | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
an | old-MSC | table | an | old-FM | cow | an | old.NTR | house | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
An old table, an old cow, an old house. |
In the singular, the form of adjectival agreement depends on gender.
As we will see, the presence or absence of agreement on AP is also sensitive to the lexical nature of the governing determiner. In addition, certain determiners itself must agree with the noun in a way that is similar to adjectival agreement, whereas other determiner may not thus agree. Thus there is a complex interaction of the lexical nature of the determiner with the singular number and with gender. The lexical nature of the determiner, number and gender all three come to play in explaining the distribution of the three paradigmatic forms of the attributive adjective. These three forms of the adjective are given below for the adjective oold ‘old’:
oold | oolde | oolden | ||
Pattern: | ZERO | -e | -en |
However, this pattern is only present with certain determiners. With other determiners, such as the definitive determiner, the adjective takes on an invariant form in schwa.
The sections below deal with agreement of AP with a preceding Determiner or a following Noun.
Both number and gender are features of nouns. Nevertheless, they do not have the same status. Number is inherently or morphologically marked on nouns:
Disk | Disk-e, | Mäme | Mäme-n, | Huus | Huz-e. | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
table | table-PL | mother | mother-PL | house | house-PL | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Table tables, mother mothers, house houses. |
The plural is in most cases marked by a schwa in the case of masculine and neuter words. Feminine words are in most cases marked by –n in the plural. This is a tendency in Saterland Frisian, which is even stronger in German. Gender, in contrast, is not marked on nouns. However, a lot of feminine nouns end in schwa. Thus there is a strong correlation between feminine gender and the nominal paradigm of schwa in the singular and –en in the plural. How, then, does gender become visible with masculine and neuter nouns, if not in the morphological form of the noun? In fact, masculine and neuter gender are distinguished contextually or syntactically, by means of the shape of a set of determiners.
The following examples show that the definite determiner makes the gender of nouns visible in the singular:
Die | Disk, | ju | Ku, | dät | Huus. | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
the | table | the | cow | the | house | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
The old table, the old cow, the old house. |
Gender is only visible in the singular. In the plural, all gender distinctions are absent. This is illustrated below for the definite determiner:
Do | Diske, | do | Mämen | do | Huze. | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
the.PL | tables | the.PL | mothers | the.PL | houses | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
The tables, the mothers, the houses. |
The definite determiner is special in that the plural form is not homophonous to any of the singular forms. Its morphological paradigm thus includes four distinct forms: die – ju – dät – do.
In the examples below, it is the proximate demonstrative article which signals the gender of the noun in the singular:
Dusse | Disk, | dusse | Mure, | dut | Huus. | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
this.MSC | table | this.FEM | wall | this.NTR | house | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
This table, this wall, this house. |
Dusse | Diske, | dusse | Muren, | dusse | Huze. | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
these.PL | tables | these.PL | walls | these.PL | houses | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
These tables, these walls, these houses. |
In contrast to the paradigm of the definite article, the paradigm of the proximate demonstrative merely features two forms: dusse – dut. These two forms are used to distinguish the masculine singular from the neuter singular (and the neuter singular from the neuter plural). So the form of the demonstrative in schwa is used for masculine and feminine singular and for all plurals. The other form is exclusively reserved for the neuter singular.
Below follows the list of determiners making gender wholly or partly visible (necessarily in the singular) on themselves:
List of determiners making a gender distinction on themselves | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
The definite article | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
The demonstrative | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
The full indefinite article (which is homophonous to the numeral one) | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
The negative article naan ‘no’ | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
The possessive pronouns | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
The determiner monig ‘many a’ (in case it is not followed by the indefinite article) |
To sum up:
- Gender and number are basic lexical features of the noun
- Number is visible on the noun and on some determiners
- Gender is primarily visible on some determiners
- Gender distinctions exist only in the singular
Keeping this in mind, we can discuss the way in which attributive adjectives agree for gender and number, or fail to do so.
Although the noun is the source of number and gender, it is the determiner which determines whether or not the adjective exhibits agreement at all. Put differently, adjectival agreement is sensitive to the nature of the governing functional head. With certain determiners, adjectival agreement for gender is obligatory, and with others is ia absent. If there is no agreement, the adjectives takes on an invariant form marked with schwa. Consider first the case of an adjective governed by the definite determiner. The determiner itself agrees with the noun for number and gender. The adjective, in contrast, does not display any agreement and it takes on the invariant form, as illustrated below:
Die | oolde | Disk, | ju | oolde | Ku | dät | oolde | Huus. | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
the | old.E | table | the | old.E | cow | the | old.E | house | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
The old table, the old cow, the old house. |
Do | oolde | Diske, | do | oolde | Bäiste | do | oolde | Huze. | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
the | old.E | tables | the | old.E | cows | the | old.E | houses | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
The old tables, the old cows, the old houses. |
The adjective is marked with a final schwa, indicated in the gloss as a capital E when it is relevant. This type of unary agreement, that is, the lack of expression of gender (or any other feature), is also referred to in the literature as weak agreement. Basically, this is a misnomer, since there is no agreement for any clear-cut feature, nor is there a paradigm. At most, this –E expresses the absence of any preceding determiner triggering strong agreement. The phenomenon is peculiar because the appearance and expression of gender agreement in the singular depends on the type of determiner that is present, whereas the features themselves come from the noun. The following table summarises the main generalisations about adjectival agreement in the singular, which we will discuss :
Table 1. Gender agreement on determiners and adjectives
Preceding determiner | Gender agreement on determiner | Gender agreement on adjective |
ZERO indefinite | - | + |
Indefinite article ‘n | - | + |
Interrogative article wo how | - | + |
Indefinite numeral / article aan | + | + |
Negative article naan | + | + |
Monich many (a) | + | + |
Su(k) ‘n such | + | + |
Possessive pronoun | ~ | - (+) |
Possessor NP | - | - |
älke each | - | - |
Definite article, demonstrative | + | - |
The word monich ‘many a’ can be followed by the indefinite numeral. If the indefinite numeral is present, monich does not show gender agreement but the indefinite numeral does. If the indefinite numeral does not accompany monich, then monich itself exhibits gender agreement. The full indefinite article aan ‘one’ is homophonous to the numeral aan ‘one’, which both trigger agreement on the following AP.
In contrast to the definite article and to the full indefinite article, the reduced indefinite article does not signal gender. It is invariantly: ‘n, a schwa followed by a nasal. The gender distinction or the gender agreement appears on the adjective:
‘n | Oold-en | Disk, | ‘n | oold-e | Ku, | ‘n | oold | Huus. | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
an | old-MSC | table | an | old-FM | cow | an | old.NTR | house | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
An old table, an old cow, an old house. |
Instead of ‘n, feminine nouns regularly feature ‘ne. The appearance of gender agreement on the adjective (or article or determiner) is also referred to in the literature as strong agreement.
If there is no determiner (ZERO in the table above), gender agreement appears on the adjective. The examples below involve singular mass nouns. These do not feature any indefinite article, nor is there any other determiner present. The adjective, so to speak, introduces the NP as a whole, and, correspondingly, there is gender agreement (strong agreement):
Roden | Wien | un | koold | Woater. | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
red.MSC | wine.MSC | and | cold.NTR | water.NTR | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Red wine and cold water. |
Gender agreement on the AP takes place only if ZERO is actually interpreted as indefinite. If ZERO is interpreted as definite, then there is no agreement, and the AP exhibits the constructional attributive form in schwa. That is the reason we may find examples like the following on signs:
Seelterske | Kultuurhuus. | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Saterlandic | culture.house | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
The Saterland house of culture. |
The question then arises under which conditions can the definite article remain absent. This may involve the naming function of signs. The following example features an indefinite article, and, correspondingly, the adjective displays gender agreement:
Dät | waas | daach | ‘n | groten | Foardeel | wezen. | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
that | was | still | a | great.MSC | advantage.MSC | been | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
That would nevertheless have been a big advantage. |
In fact, the adjective expresses both gender and number agreement in the case of masculine and neuter gender singular, seeing that the plural has a form in those genders that is different from the singular. The interrogative determiner wo ‘how’ likewise doesn’t exhibit agreement itself, but it triggers agreement on a following AP, as shown below for the MSC SG:
Wo | groten | Bak | waas | dät? | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
how | big.MSC.SG | container.MSC.SG | was | that | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
How big a container was it? |
It can be gleaned from the table that it is not the case that the absence of gender agreement on the determiner triggers its presence on the adjective. Admittedly, this holds for the indefinite article and the interrogative article: they don’t bear agreement themselves, but they trigger agreement on the adjective. This does not hold of determiners in general. The determiner älke ‘each’ is like the indefinite article in not bearing agreement itself, but neither does it trigger agreement on AP. This is shown below
Uur | älke | loze | Woud, | dät | do | Moanskene | bale. | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
about | each.E | idle.E | word.NTR | which | the | people | say | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
About each idle word which people say. |
The adjective does not have the strong agreement form for the neuter singular. So, weak adjectival agreement can also be found following determiners which themselves do not show agreement, such as the determiner älke ‘each’. The case of älke ‘each’ is interesting cross-linguistically, since it does trigger gender agreement both on itself and on the following adjective in West Frisian and Dutch. It is also interestingly that it is the form älke, and not the form älk, which has become the unchanging form used in attributive position in Saterland Frisian. It suggests that disyllabic determiners ending in schwa correlate with lack of gender agreement, so with the presence of adjectives in schwa. Nevertheless, the actual data about älke ‘each’ are more complicated than suggested here in ways not well understood and possibly involving interference.
Two cases deserve further mention because of their special behaviour. In the first place, possessive pronouns themselves display an impoverished form of gender agreement, distinguishing between masculine and the other two genders, so a two-way distinction which doesn’t focus on the neuter gender:
Min | Bruur, | mien | Suster, | mien | Huus. | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
my.MSC | brother | my | sister | my | house | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
My brother, my sister, my house. |
Mien | Brure, | mien | Sustere, | mien | Huze. | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
my.MSC | brothers | my | sisters | my | houses | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
My brothers, my sisters, my houses. |
This agreement is impoverished, since, unlike the agreement on the definite article, it conflates neuter gender with another gender, more precisely, with feminine gender.
However, there is some dialectal variation and variation over time, both with respect to the agreement on the possessive pronoun itself and on the agreement of a following adjective. First, the possessive pronoun itself shows more extensive agreement in the village Skäddel (German: Scharrel), marking the distinction between feminine and neuter by the presence or absence of a schwa. Second, there is variation in the adjectival agreement following the possessive pronoun. It tends to be weak, as in the following example:
Min | goude | Frjuund. | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
my.MSC | good.E | friend | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
My good friend. |
Strong adjectival agreement would have been expressed by the ending –en. However, it is not unusual to find strong agreement following a possessive pronoun, so the system seems to be changing here.
Finally, the interrogative adjective wäkker ‘which’ shows an impoverished gender apardigm, that is, not a three way distinction in the singular as does the definite article, but a two-way distinction. The feminine gender is distinguished as against masculine and neuter. To illustrate, consider the following example:
Wäkker | Suun, | wäkke | Dochter, | wäkker | Bouk. | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
which | son | which.FEM | daughter | which | book | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Which son, which daughter, which book. |
Incidentally, the plural form is wäkke ‘which’, so homophonous to the feminine singular form. An alternative neuter form is wäkket ‘which’. In addition, the form wäkker can also be used as the default form in attributive position, even with feminine nouns like Mäme ‘mother’, so there is variation here.
Adjectives ending in –er used to never show any adjectival agreement. This includes the words linker ‘left’ and gjuchter ‘right’. However, younger speakers tend to use them with gender agreement, in the same way as wäkke ‘which’. Other adjectives in –er which do not show any adjectival agreement are geographical adjectives. It is unclear whether younger speakers drop the –r in the feminine and the plural as well. West Frisian and Dutch do not display adjectival agreement on adjectives in –er, including geographical ones. This suggests that the Saterland pattern is a recent one, as supported by Fort’s claim that it is found especially with younger speakers. This in turn suggests that it may be an interference from Low or High German.
To sum, lack of gender agreement appears on adjectives following a group mainly consisting of some definite and indefinite determiners. The definite elements include: the definite article, the demonstratives, possessive pronouns and possessor NPs. The indefinite elements include the interrogative adjective wäkker ‘which’, the indefinite article, the negative article and the numeral aan ‘one’ and the words monich ‘many a’ and suk ‘such’.
Agreement on the adjective is never sensitive to the distinction between nominative and non-nominative arguments. Agreement on the determiner may be sensitive to the distinction between nominative and non-nominative, but only with the definite and demonstrative articles. This distinction is only present with the masculine singular:
Table 2. The distinction nominative versus non-nominative (restricted to the MSC SG of definite and demonstrative articles)
NOMINATIVE [MSC SG] | NON-NOMINATIVE | translation |
die | dän | ‘the’ |
dusse | dussen | ‘this |
krie | krän | ‘that’ |
Put differently, the only determiners exhibiting non-nominative case (marked with –n) are those which meet the following condition:
- They exhibit a visible gender distinction Neuter versus Non-Neuter in the Singular of the Nominative.
The definite article and the proximate and distal demonstratives are the only three determiners having this characteristic. The characteristic presupposes that there is some connection between case and gender. There is indeed a cross-linguistic generalisation saying that if nominative and accusative case are identical in any gender, it will always include the neuter gender. This generalisation holds true, for example, of all known Indo-European languages. An example of two distinct non-nominatives is given below:
Mäd | dussen | Wäänt | kon | me | beter | bale | as | mäd | dän. | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
with | this.NOM | guy | can | one | better | talk | than | with | that | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
This guy is easier to talk to than that one. |
The form dän clearly functions here as a demonstrative pronoun used without a following noun. This is because the distal demonstrative developed into the definite article, like in Middle West Frisian. Saterland Frisian developed a new series of distal demonstratives based on the root kr-, which derives from the expression kiek ‘see’ followed by the weakened demonstrative / definite article. This phrase developed into the root kr-, which thus received the same paradigm as the definite article:
Table 3. The distal demonstrative and its similarity to the definite article
‘that, those’ | MSC SG | FEM SG | NTR SG | PL |
NOM | krie | krju | krät | kro |
NNOM | krän | krju | krät | kro |
‘the’ | ||||
NOM | die | ju | dät | do |
NNOM | dän | ju | dät | do |
The following example contrasts the proximate demonstrative with the distal one:
Dusse | Mon | nit, | man | krie. | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
this.NOM | man | not | but | that.NOM | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Not this man, but that one. |
Cases of ellipsis remind us of the dual function of definite article, such as example (21) above, and the example below:
Ju | Seke | is | ju. | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
the | case | is | this | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
The case is as follows. |
The numerals one, two and three are special in Saterland Frisian in that they all three of them bear agreement, making a distinction between the masculine on the one hand, and the feminine and the feminine and neuter, on the other hand. This is all the more surprising for the numerals two and three, since they make this gender distinction in the plural! This was also the case in Old Germanic languages like Old English, but it is remarkable that it survives to this day.
Table 4. A gender distinction in the plural of the numerals ‘two’ and ‘three’
MSC | FEM + NTR | |
2 | twäin | two |
3 | träi | trjo |
1 | aan | een |
‘no’ | naan | neen |
Remember that gender is never marked in the plural on anything: apparently, the numerals two and three constitute an exception to this robust generalisation. Furthermore, the determiner naan / neen ‘no’ may take a singular or a plural. The question arises what happens when a plural masculine word follows this determiner. Will it follow the normal pattern, in which the plural is homophonous to the feminine singular? Or will it follow the pattern of the numbers 2, 3, which distinguish masculine gender in the plural? As it turns out, naan ‘no’ conforms to the normal pattern, not making a gender distinction in the plural. To illustrate, the following examples illustrate this claim:
Naan | Huund, | neen | Laampe, | neen | Huus. | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
no | dog.MSC.SG | no | lamp.FEM.SG | no | house.NTR.SG | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
No dog, no lamp, no house. |
Neen | Huunde, | neen | Laampen, | neen | Huze. | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
no | dog.MSC.PL | no | lamp.FEM.PL | no | house.NTR.PL | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
No dogs, no lamps, no houses. |
To return to agreement on determiners and adjectives, the chief generalisation seems to be as follows. Determiners which themselves mark the distinction between masculine versus the other two genders trigger 3-way adjectival agreement. Possessors seem to be beginning to conform to this generalisation. Determiners which themselves mark a distinction between neuter and non-neuter (definite and demonstrative articles) do not trigger adjectival agreement, nor do determiners which do not exhibit any gender distinction (such as älke ‘each’).