- Dutch
- Frisian
- Saterfrisian
- Afrikaans
-
- Phonology
- Segment inventory
- Phonotactics
- Phonological processes
- Phonology-morphology interface
- Word stress
- Primary stress in simplex words
- Monomorphemic words
- Diachronic aspects
- Generalizations on stress placement
- Default penultimate stress
- Lexical stress
- The closed penult restriction
- Final closed syllables
- The diphthong restriction
- Superheavy syllables (SHS)
- The three-syllable window
- Segmental restrictions
- Phonetic correlates
- Stress shifts in loanwords
- Quantity-sensitivity
- Secondary stress
- Vowel reduction in unstressed syllables
- Stress in complex words
- Primary stress in simplex words
- Accent & intonation
- Clitics
- Spelling
- Morphology
- Word formation
- Compounding
- Nominal compounds
- Verbal compounds
- Adjectival compounds
- Affixoids
- Coordinative compounds
- Synthetic compounds
- Reduplicative compounds
- Phrase-based compounds
- Elative compounds
- Exocentric compounds
- Linking elements
- Separable complex verbs (SCVs)
- Gapping of complex words
- Particle verbs
- Copulative compounds
- Derivation
- Numerals
- Derivation: inputs and input restrictions
- The meaning of affixes
- Non-native morphology
- Cohering and non-cohering affixes
- Prefixation
- Suffixation
- Nominal suffixation: person nouns
- Conversion
- Pseudo-participles
- Bound forms
- Nouns
- Nominal prefixes
- Nominal suffixes
- -aal and -eel
- -aar
- -aard
- -aat
- -air
- -aris
- -ast
- Diminutives
- -dom
- -een
- -ees
- -el (nominal)
- -elaar
- -enis
- -er (nominal)
- -erd
- -erik
- -es
- -eur
- -euse
- ge...te
- -heid
- -iaan, -aan
- -ief
- -iek
- -ier
- -ier (French)
- -ière
- -iet
- -igheid
- -ij and allomorphs
- -ijn
- -in
- -ing
- -isme
- -ist
- -iteit
- -ling
- -oir
- -oot
- -rice
- -schap
- -schap (de)
- -schap (het)
- -sel
- -st
- -ster
- -t
- -tal
- -te
- -voud
- Verbs
- Adjectives
- Adverbs
- Univerbation
- Neo-classical word formation
- Construction-dependent morphology
- Morphological productivity
- Compounding
- Inflection
- Inflection and derivation
- Allomorphy
- The interface between phonology and morphology
- Word formation
- Syntax
- Preface and acknowledgements
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
- 1 Characterization and classification
- 2 Projection of verb phrases I:Argument structure
- 3 Projection of verb phrases II:Verb frame alternations
- Introduction
- 3.1. Main types
- 3.2. Alternations involving the external argument
- 3.3. Alternations of noun phrases and PPs
- 3.3.1. Dative/PP alternations (dative shift)
- 3.3.1.1. Dative alternation with aan-phrases (recipients)
- 3.3.1.2. Dative alternation with naar-phrases (goals)
- 3.3.1.3. Dative alternation with van-phrases (sources)
- 3.3.1.4. Dative alternation with bij-phrases (possessors)
- 3.3.1.5. Dative alternation with voor-phrases (benefactives)
- 3.3.1.6. Conclusion
- 3.3.1.7. Bibliographical notes
- 3.3.2. Accusative/PP alternations
- 3.3.3. Nominative/PP alternations
- 3.3.1. Dative/PP alternations (dative shift)
- 3.4. Some apparent cases of verb frame alternation
- 3.5. Bibliographical notes
- 4 Projection of verb phrases IIIa:Selection of clauses/verb phrases
- 5 Projection of verb phrases IIIb:Argument and complementive clauses
- Introduction
- 5.1. Finite argument clauses
- 5.2. Infinitival argument clauses
- 5.3. Complementive clauses
- 6 Projection of verb phrases IIIc:Complements of non-main verbs
- 7 Projection of verb phrases IIId:Verb clusters
- 8 Projection of verb phrases IV: Adverbial modification
- 9 Word order in the clause I:General introduction
- 10 Word order in the clause II:Position of the finite verb (verb-first/second)
- 11 Word order in the clause III:Clause-initial position (wh-movement)
- Introduction
- 11.1. The formation of V1- and V2-clauses
- 11.2. Clause-initial position remains (phonetically) empty
- 11.3. Clause-initial position is filled
- 12 Word order in the clause IV:Postverbal field (extraposition)
- 13 Word order in the clause V: Middle field (scrambling)
- 14 Main-clause external elements
- Nouns and Noun Phrases
- 1 Characterization and classification
- 2 Projection of noun phrases I: complementation
- Introduction
- 2.1. General observations
- 2.2. Prepositional and nominal complements
- 2.3. Clausal complements
- 2.4. Bibliographical notes
- 3 Projection of noun phrases II: modification
- Introduction
- 3.1. Restrictive and non-restrictive modifiers
- 3.2. Premodification
- 3.3. Postmodification
- 3.3.1. Adpositional phrases
- 3.3.2. Relative clauses
- 3.3.3. Infinitival clauses
- 3.3.4. A special case: clauses referring to a proposition
- 3.3.5. Adjectival phrases
- 3.3.6. Adverbial postmodification
- 3.4. Bibliographical notes
- 4 Projection of noun phrases III: binominal constructions
- Introduction
- 4.1. Binominal constructions without a preposition
- 4.2. Binominal constructions with a preposition
- 4.3. Bibliographical notes
- 5 Determiners: articles and pronouns
- Introduction
- 5.1. Articles
- 5.2. Pronouns
- 5.3. Bibliographical notes
- 6 Numerals and quantifiers
- 7 Pre-determiners
- Introduction
- 7.1. The universal quantifier al 'all' and its alternants
- 7.2. The pre-determiner heel 'all/whole'
- 7.3. A note on focus particles
- 7.4. Bibliographical notes
- 8 Syntactic uses of noun phrases
- Adjectives and Adjective Phrases
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- 2 Projection of adjective phrases I: Complementation
- 3 Projection of adjective phrases II: Modification
- 4 Projection of adjective phrases III: Comparison
- 5 Attributive use of the adjective phrase
- 6 Predicative use of the adjective phrase
- 7 The partitive genitive construction
- 8 Adverbial use of the adjective phrase
- 9 Participles and infinitives: their adjectival use
- 10 Special constructions
- Adpositions and adpositional phrases
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- Introduction
- 1.1. Characterization of the category adposition
- 1.2. A formal classification of adpositional phrases
- 1.3. A semantic classification of adpositional phrases
- 1.3.1. Spatial adpositions
- 1.3.2. Temporal adpositions
- 1.3.3. Non-spatial/temporal prepositions
- 1.4. Borderline cases
- 1.5. Bibliographical notes
- 2 Projection of adpositional phrases: Complementation
- 3 Projection of adpositional phrases: Modification
- 4 Syntactic uses of the adpositional phrase
- 5 R-pronominalization and R-words
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- Phonology
-
- General
- Phonology
- Segment inventory
- Phonotactics
- Phonological Processes
- Assimilation
- Vowel nasalization
- Syllabic sonorants
- Final devoicing
- Fake geminates
- Vowel hiatus resolution
- Vowel reduction introduction
- Schwa deletion
- Schwa insertion
- /r/-deletion
- d-insertion
- {s/z}-insertion
- t-deletion
- Intrusive stop formation
- Breaking
- Vowel shortening
- h-deletion
- Replacement of the glide w
- Word stress
- Clitics
- Allomorphy
- Orthography of Frisian
- Morphology
- Inflection
- Word formation
- Derivation
- Prefixation
- Infixation
- Suffixation
- Nominal suffixes
- Verbal suffixes
- Adjectival suffixes
- Adverbial suffixes
- Numeral suffixes
- Interjectional suffixes
- Onomastic suffixes
- Conversion
- Compositions
- Derivation
- Syntax
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Unergative and unaccusative subjects
- Evidentiality
- To-infinitival clauses
- Predication and noun incorporation
- Ellipsis
- Imperativus-pro-Infinitivo
- Expression of irrealis
- Embedded Verb Second
- Agreement
- Negation
- Nouns & Noun Phrases
- Classification
- Complementation
- Modification
- Partitive noun constructions
- Referential partitive constructions
- Partitive measure nouns
- Numeral partitive constructions
- Partitive question constructions
- Nominalised quantifiers
- Kind partitives
- Partitive predication with prepositions
- Bare nominal attributions
- Articles and names
- Pronouns
- Quantifiers and (pre)determiners
- Interrogative pronouns
- R-pronouns
- Syntactic uses
- Adjective Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Complementation
- Modification and degree quantification
- Comparison by degree
- Comparative
- Superlative
- Equative
- Attribution
- Agreement
- Attributive adjectives vs. prenominal elements
- Complex adjectives
- Noun ellipsis
- Co-occurring adjectives
- Predication
- Partitive adjective constructions
- Adverbial use
- Participles and infinitives
- Adposition Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Complementation
- Modification
- Intransitive adpositions
- Predication
- Preposition stranding
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
-
- General
- Morphology
- Morphology
- 1 Word formation
- 1.1 Compounding
- 1.1.1 Compounds and their heads
- 1.1.2 Special types of compounds
- 1.1.2.1 Affixoids
- 1.1.2.2 Coordinative compounds
- 1.1.2.3 Synthetic compounds and complex pseudo-participles
- 1.1.2.4 Reduplicative compounds
- 1.1.2.5 Phrase-based compounds
- 1.1.2.6 Elative compounds
- 1.1.2.7 Exocentric compounds
- 1.1.2.8 Linking elements
- 1.1.2.9 Separable Complex Verbs and Particle Verbs
- 1.1.2.10 Noun Incorporation Verbs
- 1.1.2.11 Gapping
- 1.2 Derivation
- 1.3 Minor patterns of word formation
- 1.1 Compounding
- 2 Inflection
- 1 Word formation
- Morphology
- Syntax
- Adjectives and adjective phrases (APs)
- 0 Introduction to the AP
- 1 Characteristics and classification of APs
- 2 Complementation of APs
- 3 Modification and degree quantification of APs
- 4 Comparison by comparative, superlative and equative
- 5 Attribution of APs
- 6 Predication of APs
- 7 The partitive adjective construction
- 8 Adverbial use of APs
- 9 Participles and infinitives as APs
- Nouns and Noun Phrases (NPs)
- 0 Introduction to the NP
- 1 Characteristics and Classification of NPs
- 2 Complementation of NPs
- 3 Modification of NPs
- 3.1 Modification of NP by Determiners and APs
- 3.2 Modification of NP by PP
- 3.3 Modification of NP by adverbial clauses
- 3.4 Modification of NP by possessors
- 3.5 Modification of NP by relative clauses
- 3.6 Modification of NP in a cleft construction
- 3.7 Free relative clauses and selected interrogative clauses
- 4 Partitive noun constructions and constructions related to them
- 4.1 The referential partitive construction
- 4.2 The partitive construction of abstract quantity
- 4.3 The numerical partitive construction
- 4.4 The partitive interrogative construction
- 4.5 Adjectival, nominal and nominalised partitive quantifiers
- 4.6 Kind partitives
- 4.7 Partitive predication with a preposition
- 4.8 Bare nominal attribution
- 5 Articles and names
- 6 Pronouns
- 7 Quantifiers, determiners and predeterminers
- 8 Interrogative pronouns
- 9 R-pronouns and the indefinite expletive
- 10 Syntactic functions of Noun Phrases
- Adpositions and Adpositional Phrases (PPs)
- 0 Introduction to the PP
- 1 Characteristics and classification of PPs
- 2 Complementation of PPs
- 3 Modification of PPs
- 4 Bare (intransitive) adpositions
- 5 Predication of PPs
- 6 Form and distribution of adpositions with respect to staticity and construction type
- 7 Adpositional complements and adverbials
- Verbs and Verb Phrases (VPs)
- 0 Introduction to the VP in Saterland Frisian
- 1 Characteristics and classification of verbs
- 2 Unergative and unaccusative subjects and the auxiliary of the perfect
- 3 Evidentiality in relation to perception and epistemicity
- 4 Types of to-infinitival constituents
- 5 Predication
- 5.1 The auxiliary of being and its selection restrictions
- 5.2 The auxiliary of going and its selection restrictions
- 5.3 The auxiliary of continuation and its selection restrictions
- 5.4 The auxiliary of coming and its selection restrictions
- 5.5 Modal auxiliaries and their selection restrictions
- 5.6 Auxiliaries of body posture and aspect and their selection restrictions
- 5.7 Transitive verbs of predication
- 5.8 The auxiliary of doing used as a semantically empty finite auxiliary
- 5.9 Supplementive predication
- 6 The verbal paradigm, irregularity and suppletion
- 7 Verb Second and the word order in main and embedded clauses
- 8 Various aspects of clause structure
- Adjectives and adjective phrases (APs)
-
- General
- Phonology
- Afrikaans phonology
- Segment inventory
- Overview of Afrikaans vowels
- The diphthongised long vowels /e/, /ø/ and /o/
- The unrounded mid-front vowel /ɛ/
- The unrounded low-central vowel /ɑ/
- The unrounded low-central vowel /a/
- The rounded mid-high back vowel /ɔ/
- The rounded high back vowel /u/
- The rounded and unrounded high front vowels /i/ and /y/
- The unrounded and rounded central vowels /ə/ and /œ/
- The diphthongs /əi/, /œy/ and /œu/
- Overview of Afrikaans consonants
- The bilabial plosives /p/ and /b/
- The alveolar plosives /t/ and /d/
- The velar plosives /k/ and /g/
- The bilabial nasal /m/
- The alveolar nasal /n/
- The velar nasal /ŋ/
- The trill /r/
- The lateral liquid /l/
- The alveolar fricative /s/
- The velar fricative /x/
- The labiodental fricatives /f/ and /v/
- The approximants /ɦ/, /j/ and /ʋ/
- Overview of Afrikaans vowels
- Word stress
- The phonetic properties of stress
- Primary stress on monomorphemic words in Afrikaans
- Background to primary stress in monomorphemes in Afrikaans
- Overview of the Main Stress Rule of Afrikaans
- The short vowels of Afrikaans
- Long vowels in monomorphemes
- Primary stress on diphthongs in monomorphemes
- Exceptions
- Stress shifts in place names
- Stress shift towards word-final position
- Stress pattern of reduplications
- Phonological processes
- Vowel related processes
- Consonant related processes
- Homorganic glide insertion
- Phonology-morphology interface
- Phonotactics
- Morphology
- Syntax
- Afrikaans syntax
- Nouns and noun phrases
- Characteristics of the NP
- Classification of nouns
- Complementation of NPs
- Modification of NPs
- Binominal and partitive constructions
- Referential partitive constructions
- Partitive measure nouns
- Numeral partitive constructions
- Partitive question constructions
- Partitive constructions with nominalised quantifiers
- Partitive predication with prepositions
- Binominal name constructions
- Binominal genitive constructions
- Bare nominal attribution
- Articles and names
- Pronouns
- Quantifiers, determiners and predeterminers
- Syntactic uses of the noun phrase
- Adjectives and adjective phrases
- Characteristics and classification of the AP
- Complementation of APs
- Modification and Degree Quantification of APs
- Comparison by comparative, superlative and equative degree
- Attribution of APs
- Predication of APs
- The partitive adjective construction
- Adverbial use of APs
- Participles and infinitives as adjectives
- Verbs and verb phrases
- Characterisation and classification
- Argument structure
- Verb frame alternations
- Complements of non-main verbs
- Verb clusters
- Complement clauses
- Adverbial modification
- Word order in the clause: Introduction
- Word order in the clause: position of the finite Verb
- Word order in the clause: Clause-initial position
- Word order in the clause: Extraposition and right-dislocation in the postverbal field
- Word order in the middle field
- Emphatic constructions
- Adpositions and adposition phrases
Prepositional phrases are normally not readily eligible for adverbial modification. Still, Subsections I and II will show that this is possible with some locational, directional and temporal PPs, subsection III will discuss a set of idiomatic prepositional phrases that behave quite exceptionally with respect to adverbial modification.
Consider the locational phrases in (76). That the modifiers of the PPs, dicht'close' and hoog'high', are adjectives is clear from the fact that they can be modified by the intensifiers zeer'very' and vrij'rather', as shown in the primeless examples. The primed examples also support this claim by showing that these modifiers are also eligible for comparative and superlative formation.
a. | Peter woont | (zeer/vrij) | dicht | bij het paleis. | |
Peter lives | very/rather | close | to the palace |
a'. | Peter woont | dichter | bij het paleis | dan Jan. | |
Peter lives | closer | to the palace | than Jan |
a''. | Peter woont | het dichtst | bij het paleis. | |
Peter lives | the closest | to the palace |
b. | Jan zag | een vliegtuig | (zeer/vrij) | hoog in de lucht. | |
Jan saw | an airplane | very/rather | high in the sky |
b'. | ? | Jan zag | een vliegtuig | hoger | in de lucht. |
Jan saw | an airplane | higher | in the sky |
b''. | ?? | Jan zag een vliegtuig | het hoogst | in de lucht. |
Jan saw an airplane | the highest | in the sky |
The modifying adjectives and PPs in (76) make up constituents, which is clear from the fact that they can be placed in clause-initial position together; cf. the constituency test. Some examples that correspond to the primeless examples in (76) are given in (77); note that we have replaced the proper noun Jan in (76a) by an indefinite noun phrase in order to facilitate topicalization of the modified PP.
a. | Dicht bij het paleis | woonde | een oude schoenmaker. | |
close to the palace | lived | an old shoemaker |
b. | Hoog in de lucht | zag | Jan een vliegtuig. | |
high in the sky | saw | Jan an airplane |
The examples in (78) make clear that it is the adjective that modifies the PPs in the examples above, and not vice versa, by showing that omission of the adjective gives rise to an acceptable sentence, whereas omission of the PP is impossible.
a. | Peter woont bij het paleis. |
a'. | * | Peter woont dicht. |
b. | Jan zag een vliegtuig in de lucht. |
b'. | * | Jan zag een vliegtuig hoog. |
Example (79a) provides another argument for the claim that it is the adjective that modifies the adpositional phrase, and not vice versa: postpositional phrases are normally only used as predicative complements (cf. P4.2.1), so there can be no doubt in these cases that the adjective functions as the modifier of the PP. The fact that (79b) has the same idiomatic reading as Jan zat in de put'Jan was down-hearted' can be considered as a final argument for this claim.
a. | Jan liep | diep(er) | het bos | in. | |
Jan walked | (more) deeply | the forest | into | ||
'Jan walked (more) deeply into the forest.' |
b. | Jan zat | diep | in de put. | |
Jan sat | deeply | in the well | ||
'Jan was very depressed.' |
Occasionally, however, it is not so simple to determine the modification direction. In (80a), for example, neither the adjective ver nor the PP van de bewoonde wereld can be omitted, so we cannot determine in this way what functions as the modifier in the complex phrase ver van de bewoonde wereld; we can only conclude that it is the adjective ver that modifies the PP by appealing to analogous examples such as (80b).
a. | Ver van de bewoonde wereld | leefde | een wijze kluizenaar. | |
far from the inhabited world | lived | a wise hermit |
a'. | * | Van de bewoonde wereld leefde een wijze kluizenaar. |
a''. | * | Ver leefde een wijze kluizenaar. |
b. | Ver achter de bergen | leefde | een wijze kluizenaar. | |
far behind the mountains | lived | a wise hermit |
b'. | Achter de bergen leefde een wijze kluizenaar. |
b''. | * | Ver leefde een wijze kluizenaar. |
In (81a), it is even more difficult to determine the modification direction, given that the primed examples show that the adjective and the PP can both be omitted. One argument in favor of claiming that it is the adjective hoog that acts as the modifier of the PP boven de stoel is that (81b) shows that it can be replaced by a nominal measure phrase like twee meter'two meters'; the ungrammaticality of (81b') unambiguously shows that this nominal phrase must be analyzed as a modifier and therefore shows that an analysis of (81a) according to which the adjective acts as the modifier of the PP is at least possible. The question as to whether the alternative analysis is available as well must be left to future research.
a. | Het schilderij | hangt hoog | boven de stoel. | |
the paining | hangs high | above the chair |
a'. | Het schilderij hangt hoog. |
a''. | Het schilderij hangt boven de stoel. |
b. | Het schilderij | hangt | twee meter | boven de stoel. | |
the paining | hangs | two meters | above the chair |
b'. | * | Het schilderij hangt twee meter. |
Adjectives can modify not only full adpositional phrases, but are sometimes also able to modify particles, as in dichtbij'close', veraf'far away' and ver weg'far away'. That these cases involve adjectival modification is obscured by the fact that at least the first two combinations are normally orthographically represented as a single word, which may be related to the fact that the adjective is normally obligatory; if it is dropped in examples such as (82), the result is unacceptable.
a. | Jan woont | *(dicht)bij. | |
Jan lives | close |
b. | Jan woont | *(ver)af. | |
Jan lives | far away |
c. | Jan woont | *(ver) weg. | |
Jan lives | far away |
Since we have seen in (80a) that the same thing occasionally holds for full adpositional phrases, the examples in (82) do not provide conclusive evidence in favor of compounding. That we are not dealing with compounding is clear from the examples in (83). Given the so-called Right-hand head rule, according to which the rightmost member determines the properties of the full compound, the compound analysis wrongly predicts that the modification possibilities of the complex forms in (83) reflect those of the particles; the fact that the complex forms can be modifier by heel/zeer instead reflects a property of the adjectival part.
a. | zeer dichtbij | 'very close' |
a'. | ?zeer dicht |
a''. | *zeer bij |
b. | ?heel veraf | 'very far away' |
b'. | heel ver |
b''. | *heel af |
c. | zeer ver weg | 'very far away' |
c'. | zeer ver |
c''. | *zeer weg |
That we are not dealing with compounding is also clear from the examples in (84): the primed examples show that the adjectives dicht and ver can undergo comparative and superlative formation, which would of course be impossible if the adjectives were part of compounds.
a. | dichtbij | |
close |
a'. | dichterbij | |
closer |
a''. | het dichtstbij | |
the closest |
b. | veraf | |
far away |
b'. | verderaf | |
farther away |
b''. | het verstaf | |
the farthest away |
c. | ver weg | |
far away |
c'. | verder weg | |
farther away |
c''. | het verst weg | |
the farthest away |
The examples in (85) provide a number of modifiers for which adjectival status cannot readily be demonstrated, given that they categorically resist modification by means of an intensifier and comparative/superlative formation. The adverbs in (85b-d) may also occur in attributive and predicative position with related but not identical meanings.
a. | pal | tegen | de lat | |
right | against | the lath |
d. | precies/nauwkeurig | in de roos | |
precisely | in the bullʼs.eye |
b. | vlak | naast | het doel | |
just | beside | the goal | ||
'very close to the goal' |
e. | recht | voor | het huis | |
straight | in.front.of | the house | ||
'right in front of the house' |
c. | rechtstreeks | naar | Parijs | |
straight | to | Paris |
Consider the temporal phrases in (86). That the adjectives kort'short' and lang'long' and the PPs that follow them make up a constituent is clear from the fact, illustrated in the primed examples, that they can be placed in clause-initial position together; cf. the constituency test.
a. | Jan vertrok | kort | na de voorstelling. | |
Jan left | shortly | after the performance |
a'. | Kort na de voorstelling vertrok Jan. |
b. | Jan voltooide | zijn proefschrift | lang voor de feitelijke verdediging. | |
Jan completed | his thesis | long before the actual defense |
b'. | Lang voor de feitelijke verdediging voltooide Jan zijn proefschrift. |
That the modifier is adjectival in nature is clear from the fact that it can be modified by means of an intensifier. Comparative and superlative formation, on the other hand, seem to give rise to marked results.
a. | Jan vertrok | heel kort | na de voorstelling. | |
Jan left | very shortly | after the performance |
a'. | ?? | Jan vertrok | korter/het kortst | na de voorstelling (dan Peter). |
Jan left | more/the most shortly | after the performance (than Peter) |
b. | Jan voltooide | zijn dissertatie | heel lang | voor de feitelijke verdediging. | |
Jan completed | his thesis | very long | before the actual defense |
b'. | ?? | Jan voltooide zijn dissertatie | langer/het langst | voor de feitelijke verdediging. |
Jan completed his thesis | longer/the longest | before the actual defense |
The complementizers voordat'before' and nadat'after' are compounds consisting of a temporal preposition and the complementizer dat'that'; cf. Section P2.4.1, sub II. The examples in (88) show that dependent clauses that are introduced by these complementizers can be modified in the same way as the PPs in (86). The primed examples show that the adjective and the dependent clause form a constituent.
a. | Jan vertrok | kort | nadat | de voorstelling | begon. | |
Jan left | shortly | after | the performance | started |
a'. | Kort nadat de voorstelling begon, vertrok Jan. |
b. | Jan voltooide zijn proefschrift | lang voordat | hij | het | feitelijk | moest verdedigen. | |
Jan completed his thesis | long before | he | it | actually | had to defend | ||
'Jan completed his thesis long before he actually had to defend it.' |
b'. | Lang voordat hij het feitelijk moest verdedigen, voltooide Jan zijn proefschrift. |
The examples in (89) provide a number of modifiers for which adjectival status cannot readily be demonstrated, given that they categorically resist modification by means of an intensifier and comparative/superlative formation; cf. example (85).
a. | pal/direct na de voorstelling | 'immediately after the performance' |
b. | vlak/net voor de voorstelling | 'just before the performance' |
c. | precies tijdens het begin | 'exactly at the beginning' |
Dutch has a large set of idiomatic prepositional expressions that can be used in complementive position; some examples are given in (90). That we are dealing with idioms is clear from the fact that the attributive modification of the noun in the PP is normally excluded; see Section P1.3.3, sub I for more discussion.
a. | op je gemak zijn | 'to be at oneʼs ease' |
b. | in je knollentuin/nopjes/sas/schik zijn | 'to be pleased' |
c. | van streek zijn | 'to be upset' |
The idiomatic PPs in (90) exhibit several similarities with scalar adjectives. First, they denote some (mental) property of the subject of the clause, just like adjectives such as gelukkig'happy' or tevreden'satisfied'. Second, the primeless examples in (91) show that these PPs can be modified by an intensifier; observe that the modifier may be the element zeer, which is generally assumed to co-occur with adjectives only. The primed examples are added to show that the adjectives and the PPs in the primeless examples form a constituent; cf. the constituency test.
a. | Jan is hier erg/zeer | op zijn gemak. | |
Jan is here very | at his ease | ||
'Jan is very at his ease here.' |
a'. | Erg op zijn gemak is Jan hier niet. |
b. | Jan is helemaal/zeer | in zʼn knollentuin/nopjes/sas/schik. | |
Jan is completely | in his vegetable garden/nopjes/sas/schik | ||
'Jan is very pleased.' |
b'. | Helemaal in zʼn knollentuin/nopjes/sas/schik is Jan niet. |
c. | Jan is erg/zeer | van streek. | |
Jan is very | of streek | ||
'Jan is very upset.' |
c'. | Zeer van streek is Jan niet. |
Finally, the PPs in (90) seem to be eligible for comparative/superlative formation just like the scalar adjectives. This is shown in (92).
a. | Jan is hier meer | op zijn gemak | dan Peter. | |
Jan is here more | at his ease | than Peter |
a'. | Jan is hier | het meest | op zijn gemak. | |
Jan is here | the most | at his ease |
b. | Jan is meer | in zʼn knollentuin/nopjes/sas/schik | dan Peter. | |
Jan is more | in his vegetable garden/nopjes/sas/schik | than Peter | ||
'Jan is more pleased than Peter.' |
b'. | Jan is het meest | in zʼn knollentuin/nopjes/sas/schik. | |
Jan is the most | in his vegetable garden/nopjes/sas/schik |
c. | Jan is meer | van streek | dan Peter. | |
Jan is more | of streek | than Peter | ||
'Jan is more upset than Peter.' |
c'. | Jan is het meest | van streek. | |
Jan is the most | of streek |