- Dutch
- Frisian
- Saterfrisian
- Afrikaans
-
- Phonology
- Segment inventory
- Phonotactics
- Phonological processes
- Phonology-morphology interface
- Word stress
- Primary stress in simplex words
- Monomorphemic words
- Diachronic aspects
- Generalizations on stress placement
- Default penultimate stress
- Lexical stress
- The closed penult restriction
- Final closed syllables
- The diphthong restriction
- Superheavy syllables (SHS)
- The three-syllable window
- Segmental restrictions
- Phonetic correlates
- Stress shifts in loanwords
- Quantity-sensitivity
- Secondary stress
- Vowel reduction in unstressed syllables
- Stress in complex words
- Primary stress in simplex words
- Accent & intonation
- Clitics
- Spelling
- Morphology
- Word formation
- Compounding
- Nominal compounds
- Verbal compounds
- Adjectival compounds
- Affixoids
- Coordinative compounds
- Synthetic compounds
- Reduplicative compounds
- Phrase-based compounds
- Elative compounds
- Exocentric compounds
- Linking elements
- Separable complex verbs (SCVs)
- Gapping of complex words
- Particle verbs
- Copulative compounds
- Derivation
- Numerals
- Derivation: inputs and input restrictions
- The meaning of affixes
- Non-native morphology
- Cohering and non-cohering affixes
- Prefixation
- Suffixation
- Nominal suffixation: person nouns
- Conversion
- Pseudo-participles
- Bound forms
- Nouns
- Nominal prefixes
- Nominal suffixes
- -aal and -eel
- -aar
- -aard
- -aat
- -air
- -aris
- -ast
- Diminutives
- -dom
- -een
- -ees
- -el (nominal)
- -elaar
- -enis
- -er (nominal)
- -erd
- -erik
- -es
- -eur
- -euse
- ge...te
- -heid
- -iaan, -aan
- -ief
- -iek
- -ier
- -ier (French)
- -ière
- -iet
- -igheid
- -ij and allomorphs
- -ijn
- -in
- -ing
- -isme
- -ist
- -iteit
- -ling
- -oir
- -oot
- -rice
- -schap
- -schap (de)
- -schap (het)
- -sel
- -st
- -ster
- -t
- -tal
- -te
- -voud
- Verbs
- Adjectives
- Adverbs
- Univerbation
- Neo-classical word formation
- Construction-dependent morphology
- Morphological productivity
- Compounding
- Inflection
- Inflection and derivation
- Allomorphy
- The interface between phonology and morphology
- Word formation
- Syntax
- Preface and acknowledgements
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
- 1 Characterization and classification
- 2 Projection of verb phrases I:Argument structure
- 3 Projection of verb phrases II:Verb frame alternations
- Introduction
- 3.1. Main types
- 3.2. Alternations involving the external argument
- 3.3. Alternations of noun phrases and PPs
- 3.3.1. Dative/PP alternations (dative shift)
- 3.3.1.1. Dative alternation with aan-phrases (recipients)
- 3.3.1.2. Dative alternation with naar-phrases (goals)
- 3.3.1.3. Dative alternation with van-phrases (sources)
- 3.3.1.4. Dative alternation with bij-phrases (possessors)
- 3.3.1.5. Dative alternation with voor-phrases (benefactives)
- 3.3.1.6. Conclusion
- 3.3.1.7. Bibliographical notes
- 3.3.2. Accusative/PP alternations
- 3.3.3. Nominative/PP alternations
- 3.3.1. Dative/PP alternations (dative shift)
- 3.4. Some apparent cases of verb frame alternation
- 3.5. Bibliographical notes
- 4 Projection of verb phrases IIIa:Selection of clauses/verb phrases
- 5 Projection of verb phrases IIIb:Argument and complementive clauses
- Introduction
- 5.1. Finite argument clauses
- 5.2. Infinitival argument clauses
- 5.3. Complementive clauses
- 6 Projection of verb phrases IIIc:Complements of non-main verbs
- 7 Projection of verb phrases IIId:Verb clusters
- 8 Projection of verb phrases IV: Adverbial modification
- 9 Word order in the clause I:General introduction
- 10 Word order in the clause II:Position of the finite verb (verb-first/second)
- 11 Word order in the clause III:Clause-initial position (wh-movement)
- Introduction
- 11.1. The formation of V1- and V2-clauses
- 11.2. Clause-initial position remains (phonetically) empty
- 11.3. Clause-initial position is filled
- 12 Word order in the clause IV:Postverbal field (extraposition)
- 13 Word order in the clause V: Middle field (scrambling)
- 14 Main-clause external elements
- Nouns and Noun Phrases
- 1 Characterization and classification
- 2 Projection of noun phrases I: complementation
- Introduction
- 2.1. General observations
- 2.2. Prepositional and nominal complements
- 2.3. Clausal complements
- 2.4. Bibliographical notes
- 3 Projection of noun phrases II: modification
- Introduction
- 3.1. Restrictive and non-restrictive modifiers
- 3.2. Premodification
- 3.3. Postmodification
- 3.3.1. Adpositional phrases
- 3.3.2. Relative clauses
- 3.3.3. Infinitival clauses
- 3.3.4. A special case: clauses referring to a proposition
- 3.3.5. Adjectival phrases
- 3.3.6. Adverbial postmodification
- 3.4. Bibliographical notes
- 4 Projection of noun phrases III: binominal constructions
- Introduction
- 4.1. Binominal constructions without a preposition
- 4.2. Binominal constructions with a preposition
- 4.3. Bibliographical notes
- 5 Determiners: articles and pronouns
- Introduction
- 5.1. Articles
- 5.2. Pronouns
- 5.3. Bibliographical notes
- 6 Numerals and quantifiers
- 7 Pre-determiners
- Introduction
- 7.1. The universal quantifier al 'all' and its alternants
- 7.2. The pre-determiner heel 'all/whole'
- 7.3. A note on focus particles
- 7.4. Bibliographical notes
- 8 Syntactic uses of noun phrases
- Adjectives and Adjective Phrases
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- 2 Projection of adjective phrases I: Complementation
- 3 Projection of adjective phrases II: Modification
- 4 Projection of adjective phrases III: Comparison
- 5 Attributive use of the adjective phrase
- 6 Predicative use of the adjective phrase
- 7 The partitive genitive construction
- 8 Adverbial use of the adjective phrase
- 9 Participles and infinitives: their adjectival use
- 10 Special constructions
- Adpositions and adpositional phrases
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- Introduction
- 1.1. Characterization of the category adposition
- 1.2. A formal classification of adpositional phrases
- 1.3. A semantic classification of adpositional phrases
- 1.3.1. Spatial adpositions
- 1.3.2. Temporal adpositions
- 1.3.3. Non-spatial/temporal prepositions
- 1.4. Borderline cases
- 1.5. Bibliographical notes
- 2 Projection of adpositional phrases: Complementation
- 3 Projection of adpositional phrases: Modification
- 4 Syntactic uses of the adpositional phrase
- 5 R-pronominalization and R-words
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- Phonology
-
- General
- Phonology
- Segment inventory
- Phonotactics
- Phonological Processes
- Assimilation
- Vowel nasalization
- Syllabic sonorants
- Final devoicing
- Fake geminates
- Vowel hiatus resolution
- Vowel reduction introduction
- Schwa deletion
- Schwa insertion
- /r/-deletion
- d-insertion
- {s/z}-insertion
- t-deletion
- Intrusive stop formation
- Breaking
- Vowel shortening
- h-deletion
- Replacement of the glide w
- Word stress
- Clitics
- Allomorphy
- Orthography of Frisian
- Morphology
- Inflection
- Word formation
- Derivation
- Prefixation
- Infixation
- Suffixation
- Nominal suffixes
- Verbal suffixes
- Adjectival suffixes
- Adverbial suffixes
- Numeral suffixes
- Interjectional suffixes
- Onomastic suffixes
- Conversion
- Compositions
- Derivation
- Syntax
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Unergative and unaccusative subjects
- Evidentiality
- To-infinitival clauses
- Predication and noun incorporation
- Ellipsis
- Imperativus-pro-Infinitivo
- Expression of irrealis
- Embedded Verb Second
- Agreement
- Negation
- Nouns & Noun Phrases
- Classification
- Complementation
- Modification
- Partitive noun constructions
- Referential partitive constructions
- Partitive measure nouns
- Numeral partitive constructions
- Partitive question constructions
- Nominalised quantifiers
- Kind partitives
- Partitive predication with prepositions
- Bare nominal attributions
- Articles and names
- Pronouns
- Quantifiers and (pre)determiners
- Interrogative pronouns
- R-pronouns
- Syntactic uses
- Adjective Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Complementation
- Modification and degree quantification
- Comparison by degree
- Comparative
- Superlative
- Equative
- Attribution
- Agreement
- Attributive adjectives vs. prenominal elements
- Complex adjectives
- Noun ellipsis
- Co-occurring adjectives
- Predication
- Partitive adjective constructions
- Adverbial use
- Participles and infinitives
- Adposition Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Complementation
- Modification
- Intransitive adpositions
- Predication
- Preposition stranding
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
-
- General
- Morphology
- Morphology
- 1 Word formation
- 1.1 Compounding
- 1.1.1 Compounds and their heads
- 1.1.2 Special types of compounds
- 1.1.2.1 Affixoids
- 1.1.2.2 Coordinative compounds
- 1.1.2.3 Synthetic compounds and complex pseudo-participles
- 1.1.2.4 Reduplicative compounds
- 1.1.2.5 Phrase-based compounds
- 1.1.2.6 Elative compounds
- 1.1.2.7 Exocentric compounds
- 1.1.2.8 Linking elements
- 1.1.2.9 Separable Complex Verbs and Particle Verbs
- 1.1.2.10 Noun Incorporation Verbs
- 1.1.2.11 Gapping
- 1.2 Derivation
- 1.3 Minor patterns of word formation
- 1.1 Compounding
- 2 Inflection
- 1 Word formation
- Morphology
- Syntax
- Adjectives and adjective phrases (APs)
- 0 Introduction to the AP
- 1 Characteristics and classification of APs
- 2 Complementation of APs
- 3 Modification and degree quantification of APs
- 4 Comparison by comparative, superlative and equative
- 5 Attribution of APs
- 6 Predication of APs
- 7 The partitive adjective construction
- 8 Adverbial use of APs
- 9 Participles and infinitives as APs
- Nouns and Noun Phrases (NPs)
- 0 Introduction to the NP
- 1 Characteristics and Classification of NPs
- 2 Complementation of NPs
- 3 Modification of NPs
- 3.1 Modification of NP by Determiners and APs
- 3.2 Modification of NP by PP
- 3.3 Modification of NP by adverbial clauses
- 3.4 Modification of NP by possessors
- 3.5 Modification of NP by relative clauses
- 3.6 Modification of NP in a cleft construction
- 3.7 Free relative clauses and selected interrogative clauses
- 4 Partitive noun constructions and constructions related to them
- 4.1 The referential partitive construction
- 4.2 The partitive construction of abstract quantity
- 4.3 The numerical partitive construction
- 4.4 The partitive interrogative construction
- 4.5 Adjectival, nominal and nominalised partitive quantifiers
- 4.6 Kind partitives
- 4.7 Partitive predication with a preposition
- 4.8 Bare nominal attribution
- 5 Articles and names
- 6 Pronouns
- 7 Quantifiers, determiners and predeterminers
- 8 Interrogative pronouns
- 9 R-pronouns and the indefinite expletive
- 10 Syntactic functions of Noun Phrases
- Adpositions and Adpositional Phrases (PPs)
- 0 Introduction to the PP
- 1 Characteristics and classification of PPs
- 2 Complementation of PPs
- 3 Modification of PPs
- 4 Bare (intransitive) adpositions
- 5 Predication of PPs
- 6 Form and distribution of adpositions with respect to staticity and construction type
- 7 Adpositional complements and adverbials
- Verbs and Verb Phrases (VPs)
- 0 Introduction to the VP in Saterland Frisian
- 1 Characteristics and classification of verbs
- 2 Unergative and unaccusative subjects and the auxiliary of the perfect
- 3 Evidentiality in relation to perception and epistemicity
- 4 Types of to-infinitival constituents
- 5 Predication
- 5.1 The auxiliary of being and its selection restrictions
- 5.2 The auxiliary of going and its selection restrictions
- 5.3 The auxiliary of continuation and its selection restrictions
- 5.4 The auxiliary of coming and its selection restrictions
- 5.5 Modal auxiliaries and their selection restrictions
- 5.6 Auxiliaries of body posture and aspect and their selection restrictions
- 5.7 Transitive verbs of predication
- 5.8 The auxiliary of doing used as a semantically empty finite auxiliary
- 5.9 Supplementive predication
- 6 The verbal paradigm, irregularity and suppletion
- 7 Verb Second and the word order in main and embedded clauses
- 8 Various aspects of clause structure
- Adjectives and adjective phrases (APs)
-
- General
- Phonology
- Afrikaans phonology
- Segment inventory
- Overview of Afrikaans vowels
- The diphthongised long vowels /e/, /ø/ and /o/
- The unrounded mid-front vowel /ɛ/
- The unrounded low-central vowel /ɑ/
- The unrounded low-central vowel /a/
- The rounded mid-high back vowel /ɔ/
- The rounded high back vowel /u/
- The rounded and unrounded high front vowels /i/ and /y/
- The unrounded and rounded central vowels /ə/ and /œ/
- The diphthongs /əi/, /œy/ and /œu/
- Overview of Afrikaans consonants
- The bilabial plosives /p/ and /b/
- The alveolar plosives /t/ and /d/
- The velar plosives /k/ and /g/
- The bilabial nasal /m/
- The alveolar nasal /n/
- The velar nasal /ŋ/
- The trill /r/
- The lateral liquid /l/
- The alveolar fricative /s/
- The velar fricative /x/
- The labiodental fricatives /f/ and /v/
- The approximants /ɦ/, /j/ and /ʋ/
- Overview of Afrikaans vowels
- Word stress
- The phonetic properties of stress
- Primary stress on monomorphemic words in Afrikaans
- Background to primary stress in monomorphemes in Afrikaans
- Overview of the Main Stress Rule of Afrikaans
- The short vowels of Afrikaans
- Long vowels in monomorphemes
- Primary stress on diphthongs in monomorphemes
- Exceptions
- Stress shifts in place names
- Stress shift towards word-final position
- Stress pattern of reduplications
- Phonological processes
- Vowel related processes
- Consonant related processes
- Homorganic glide insertion
- Phonology-morphology interface
- Phonotactics
- Morphology
- Syntax
- Afrikaans syntax
- Nouns and noun phrases
- Characteristics of the NP
- Classification of nouns
- Complementation of NPs
- Modification of NPs
- Binominal and partitive constructions
- Referential partitive constructions
- Partitive measure nouns
- Numeral partitive constructions
- Partitive question constructions
- Partitive constructions with nominalised quantifiers
- Partitive predication with prepositions
- Binominal name constructions
- Binominal genitive constructions
- Bare nominal attribution
- Articles and names
- Pronouns
- Quantifiers, determiners and predeterminers
- Syntactic uses of the noun phrase
- Adjectives and adjective phrases
- Characteristics and classification of the AP
- Complementation of APs
- Modification and Degree Quantification of APs
- Comparison by comparative, superlative and equative degree
- Attribution of APs
- Predication of APs
- The partitive adjective construction
- Adverbial use of APs
- Participles and infinitives as adjectives
- Verbs and verb phrases
- Characterisation and classification
- Argument structure
- Verb frame alternations
- Complements of non-main verbs
- Verb clusters
- Complement clauses
- Adverbial modification
- Word order in the clause: Introduction
- Word order in the clause: position of the finite Verb
- Word order in the clause: Clause-initial position
- Word order in the clause: Extraposition and right-dislocation in the postverbal field
- Word order in the middle field
- Emphatic constructions
- Adpositions and adposition phrases
Consonant clusters of /s/ and a voiceless plosive — so-called 'complex segments' — act as single phonological units, for which several arguments can be adduced. They are the topic of this section.
Consonant clusters of /s/ and a voiceless plosive act as single phonological units.
A first peculiarity of these clusters is that they can occur both word-initially and word-finally, with the obstruents appearing in either order (see clusters of /s/ + voiceless plosive in word-initial position and clusters of /s/ + voiceless plosive in word-final position). They do not seem to conform to the Sonority Sequencing Constraint, in marked contrast with clusters of an obstruent and a sonorant consonant, which are each other's mirror image in word-initial and word-final position.
There are two more pieces of evidence that these clusters act as units. In the first place, it is observed in Hoekstra (1985:70-71) that degemination (see degemination) not only affects double consonants, but also double /s/ + plosive clusters, which only show up in compounds and phrases, as shown in (1):
list#stik | [st] | piece of a frame |
fisk#skaal | [sk] | fish dish |
de gasp spanne | [sp] | to tighten the buckle |
op 'e kast stean | [st] | to stand on the cupboard |
de wask skjin | [sk] | the laundry clean |
Geps psalmboek | [ps] | Gep's psalmbook |
oer de nije fyts tsiere | [ts] | to quarrel about the new bicycle |
is dat faaks ksenon | [ks] | does this happen to be xenon? |
For degemination to occur, these sequences must have the status of a single unit at some level of representation.
In the second place, /s/ + plosive clusters cannot be split by schwa-epenthesis, whereas a branching onset, of an obstruent and a liquid, can (see schwa insertion in onset clusters). So, trien /triən/ tear can be pronounced as [təri.ən], but tsien /tsiən/ ten or stien /stiən/ stone not as [*təsi.ən] or [*səti.ən]. Insertion would disrupt the unit which the sequence of /s/ + plosive is.
The /s/+ plosive clusters are not complex segments at the level of underlying representation, i.e. they are the result of phonological processing, for which several arguments can be adduced.
One argument of formal economy is that if complex segments arise through processing, the phoneme inventory of Frisian can be kept much simpler than when they are assigned phonemic status.
Another argument is that these sequences also show up in morphologically derived structures. There are three cases at hand. First, the possessive suffix -s ( /-s/) can be attached to a proper name ending in /-p/, /-t/, or /-k/ in order to mark the possessor of the object denoted by the following noun (see the possessive suffix -s), as in Geps /ɡɛp+s/ Gep's, Juks /jøk+s/ Juk's, and Reits /rajt+s/ Reit's. Second, the suffix -s ( /-s/) can also be attached to an adjective ending in /-p/, /-t/, or /-k/ to form a 'partitive genitive' (see partitive construction), as in wat knaps /knap+s/ something smart, wat wyts /vit+s/ something white, and wat bryks /brik+s/ something crooked. Third, the third person singular present tense verbal suffix -t ( /-t/) can be attached to a verb stem (of the first weak or strong/irregular class, see paradigm of class I) ending in /-s/, as in hy/hja past /pɔs+t/ [pɔst]hjir he/she belongs here.
/s/ + plosive clusters can also result from phonological processes which 'act upon' obstruent sequences derived by morphology. When a proper name ends in voiced /-b/ or /-d/, these show up as [p] or [t] when the possessive suffix is added, as in Sybs /sib+s/ [sips] Syb's or Tseards /tsɪəd+s/ [tsɪəts] Tseard's. The same happens when an adjective ending in /-b/ or /-d/ is suffixed with partitive /-s/, as in wat sibs /sɪb+s/ [sɪps] something familiar and wat reads /rɪəd+s/ [rɪəts] something read.
Verbal morphology also gives rise to the clusters at hand. When a verbal stem ends in /-z/, the latter turns into [s] upon suffixation with /-t/ (3rd person singular, present tense), as in hy/hja gniist /ɡni:z+t/ [ɡni:st] he/she smirks/sneers. The suffix -st (2nd person singular, present tense) may have the same effect, be it in an indirect way. There are two instances to be considered. First, when a verbal stem ending in /-s/ is suffixed with -st, as in do skrast you strike out, the resulting form, /skrɔs+st/, undergoes degemination (see degemination): [skrɔst]. Second, stem-final /-z/ turns into [s] before the suffix -st; with subsequent degemination this yields a form with final [-st], as in do gniist you smirk/sneer /ɡni:z+st→ ɡni:sst→ gni:st/. These are all cases of obstruent devoicing before a word-final (voiceless) obstruent.
Take a verb like lige /li:ɣ+ə/ to tell a lie. When /li:ɣ/ is suffixed with -st, stem-final /-ɣ/ is turned into [-x], resulting in [lixst]. The marked fricative sequence [-xs-] may be repaired by the dissimilatory hardening of /x/ to /k/ (see dissimilation), resulting in [likst] (for the shortening of /-i:-/ to [-i-], see overview of the contexts of Vowel Shortening). Take also the noun nacht /naxt/ night, from which the temporal adverb nachts /naxt+s/ at night derives. Deletion of /t/ turns nachts into /naxs/, the final cluster of which may undergo dissimilation: [naks] (see dissimilation).
All these /s/ + plosive or plosive + /s/ clusters, whether derived morphologically or phonologically, act as units. First, they can participate in degemination, as in the examples in (2) (cf. those in (1)):
Degemination of derived /s/ + plosive clusters | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Geps psalmboek | [ps] | Gep's psalmbook | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
wêr is Juks ksylofoan keard | [ks] | where has Juk's xylophone got to? | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
nachts | [naks] | ksylofoan spylje | [ks] | to play the xylophone at night | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
ik wit net, hoe't Jaaps psyche wurket | [ps] | I do not know how Jaap's psyche works | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
ik kaam wat wyts tsjin | [ts] | I came across something white | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
oer wat reads tsiere | [ts] | to quarrel about something red | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
dat past stilistysk | [st] | net | that does not fit stylistically | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
hy blaast stevich | [st] | he blows firmly |
Second, they cannot be split by epenthesis: Geps [*ɡɛpəs], wat wyts [*vitəs], wat reads [*rɪətəs], hy/hja past [*pɔsət], hy/hja blaast [*bla:sət], nachts [*nakəs]. The assumption that all /s/ + plosive complex segments are the result of phonological processing, hence that they are not present as such in underlying representation, enables one to arrive at a generalized analysis.
The fact that the verb forms [pɔsət] and [bla:sət] do not strike us as too bad may be ascribed to the fact that the 3rd person singular of verbs of the (unmarked) second weak class ends in [-ət] (see paradigm of class II).
Diminutive formation provides us with an argument that noun-final /-st/ is a sequence of two separate segments in underlying representation. Nouns ending in /-ft/ and /-st/, like skoft /skoft/ while, time and kast /kɔst/ cupboard, have two diminutive forms, viz. skoftsje /skoft+tsjə/ [skoftsjə] and kastje /kɔst+tsjə/ [kɔsjə], and skoftke /skof+kə/ [skofkə] and kastke /kɔs+kə/ [kɔskə]. The forms ending in [-jə] are found in the eastern part of Fryslân, those ending in [-kə] in the western part of Fryslân (see phonological adjustments). The forms skoftke and kastke arise through deletion of (extrasyllabic) stem-final /-t/, after which the diminutive allomorph -ke ( /-kə/) is selected on the basis of /-f/ or /-s/, which have become stem-final (cf. slof /slof/ slipper, mule --> slofke [slofkə] little slipper, mule and tas /tɔs/ bag --> taske [tɔskə] little bag). All this speaks in favour of an analysis in which stem-final /-st/ is a sequence of two separate segments in underlying representation, which is turned into a complex segment through phonological processing. Apart from the objections to underlying complex segments raised above, in this particular case they would force us to assume two kinds of /t/-deletion, one for the complex segment /-st/ and one for the sequence /-ft/, which would considerably and unnecessarily complicate the grammar.
Especially in the southern part of Fryslân, the initial /t/ of the sequence /tsj-/ is prone to deletion. Words like tsjerke /tsjɛrkə/ church, tsjettel /tsjɛtəl/ kettle, and tsjuster /tsjøstər/ dark; darkness are often pronounced there as [sjɛrkə], [sjɛtl̩], and [sjøstr̩].
In general, this is not reflected in the spelling, though tsjoele and sjoele shovel occur side by side.
This deletion is best accounted for if /ts-/ is analyzed as a cluster of two separate consonants.
The occurrence of syllabic sonorant consonants provides a purely phonological argument that word-final /s/ + plosive clusters consist of two separate segments in underlying representation. The final sequences in clusters of /s + plosive in word-final position are, among others, found in nouns: gasp /gɔsp/ buckle, clasp, meeps /me:ps/ wasp, least /lɪəst/ last; footmark, guts /gøts/ gouge, fisk /fɪsk/ fish, and foks /foks/ fox. When a suffix of the form schwa + sonorant consonant is attached to these stems, as in the plurals gaspen /gɔsp+ən/ buckles, clasps, meepsen /me:ps+ən/ wasps, leasten /lɪəst+ən/ lasts; footmarks, gutsen /gøts+ən/ gouges, fisk /fɪsk+ən/ fishes, and foks /foks+ən/ foxes, these can be realized as /(ɡɔs)(pm̩)/, /(me:p)(sn̩)/, /(lɪəs)(tn̩)/, /(ɡøt)(sn̩)/, /(fɪs)(kŋ̩)/, and /(fok)(sn̩)/. The syllabic nasal provides strong evidence that /s/, /p/, /t/, and /k/ are in the onset of the final syllable, since a syllable headed by a (sonorant) consonant must have an onset (see the onset condition). This, in its turn, means that the stem-final clusters in the above examples are best analyzed as two separate consonants, both of which are syllabified independently from each other.
- 1985t-deletion before suffix-initial st in modern West FrisianNOWELE : North-Western European language evolution563-76