- Dutch
- Frisian
- Saterfrisian
- Afrikaans
-
- Phonology
- Segment inventory
- Phonotactics
- Phonological processes
- Phonology-morphology interface
- Word stress
- Primary stress in simplex words
- Monomorphemic words
- Diachronic aspects
- Generalizations on stress placement
- Default penultimate stress
- Lexical stress
- The closed penult restriction
- Final closed syllables
- The diphthong restriction
- Superheavy syllables (SHS)
- The three-syllable window
- Segmental restrictions
- Phonetic correlates
- Stress shifts in loanwords
- Quantity-sensitivity
- Secondary stress
- Vowel reduction in unstressed syllables
- Stress in complex words
- Primary stress in simplex words
- Accent & intonation
- Clitics
- Spelling
- Morphology
- Word formation
- Compounding
- Nominal compounds
- Verbal compounds
- Adjectival compounds
- Affixoids
- Coordinative compounds
- Synthetic compounds
- Reduplicative compounds
- Phrase-based compounds
- Elative compounds
- Exocentric compounds
- Linking elements
- Separable complex verbs (SCVs)
- Gapping of complex words
- Particle verbs
- Copulative compounds
- Derivation
- Numerals
- Derivation: inputs and input restrictions
- The meaning of affixes
- Non-native morphology
- Cohering and non-cohering affixes
- Prefixation
- Suffixation
- Nominal suffixation: person nouns
- Conversion
- Pseudo-participles
- Bound forms
- Nouns
- Nominal prefixes
- Nominal suffixes
- -aal and -eel
- -aar
- -aard
- -aat
- -air
- -aris
- -ast
- Diminutives
- -dom
- -een
- -ees
- -el (nominal)
- -elaar
- -enis
- -er (nominal)
- -erd
- -erik
- -es
- -eur
- -euse
- ge...te
- -heid
- -iaan, -aan
- -ief
- -iek
- -ier
- -ier (French)
- -ière
- -iet
- -igheid
- -ij and allomorphs
- -ijn
- -in
- -ing
- -isme
- -ist
- -iteit
- -ling
- -oir
- -oot
- -rice
- -schap
- -schap (de)
- -schap (het)
- -sel
- -st
- -ster
- -t
- -tal
- -te
- -voud
- Verbs
- Adjectives
- Adverbs
- Univerbation
- Neo-classical word formation
- Construction-dependent morphology
- Morphological productivity
- Compounding
- Inflection
- Inflection and derivation
- Allomorphy
- The interface between phonology and morphology
- Word formation
- Syntax
- Preface and acknowledgements
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
- 1 Characterization and classification
- 2 Projection of verb phrases I:Argument structure
- 3 Projection of verb phrases II:Verb frame alternations
- Introduction
- 3.1. Main types
- 3.2. Alternations involving the external argument
- 3.3. Alternations of noun phrases and PPs
- 3.3.1. Dative/PP alternations (dative shift)
- 3.3.1.1. Dative alternation with aan-phrases (recipients)
- 3.3.1.2. Dative alternation with naar-phrases (goals)
- 3.3.1.3. Dative alternation with van-phrases (sources)
- 3.3.1.4. Dative alternation with bij-phrases (possessors)
- 3.3.1.5. Dative alternation with voor-phrases (benefactives)
- 3.3.1.6. Conclusion
- 3.3.1.7. Bibliographical notes
- 3.3.2. Accusative/PP alternations
- 3.3.3. Nominative/PP alternations
- 3.3.1. Dative/PP alternations (dative shift)
- 3.4. Some apparent cases of verb frame alternation
- 3.5. Bibliographical notes
- 4 Projection of verb phrases IIIa:Selection of clauses/verb phrases
- 5 Projection of verb phrases IIIb:Argument and complementive clauses
- Introduction
- 5.1. Finite argument clauses
- 5.2. Infinitival argument clauses
- 5.3. Complementive clauses
- 6 Projection of verb phrases IIIc:Complements of non-main verbs
- 7 Projection of verb phrases IIId:Verb clusters
- 8 Projection of verb phrases IV: Adverbial modification
- 9 Word order in the clause I:General introduction
- 10 Word order in the clause II:Position of the finite verb (verb-first/second)
- 11 Word order in the clause III:Clause-initial position (wh-movement)
- Introduction
- 11.1. The formation of V1- and V2-clauses
- 11.2. Clause-initial position remains (phonetically) empty
- 11.3. Clause-initial position is filled
- 12 Word order in the clause IV:Postverbal field (extraposition)
- 13 Word order in the clause V: Middle field (scrambling)
- 14 Main-clause external elements
- Nouns and Noun Phrases
- 1 Characterization and classification
- 2 Projection of noun phrases I: complementation
- Introduction
- 2.1. General observations
- 2.2. Prepositional and nominal complements
- 2.3. Clausal complements
- 2.4. Bibliographical notes
- 3 Projection of noun phrases II: modification
- Introduction
- 3.1. Restrictive and non-restrictive modifiers
- 3.2. Premodification
- 3.3. Postmodification
- 3.3.1. Adpositional phrases
- 3.3.2. Relative clauses
- 3.3.3. Infinitival clauses
- 3.3.4. A special case: clauses referring to a proposition
- 3.3.5. Adjectival phrases
- 3.3.6. Adverbial postmodification
- 3.4. Bibliographical notes
- 4 Projection of noun phrases III: binominal constructions
- Introduction
- 4.1. Binominal constructions without a preposition
- 4.2. Binominal constructions with a preposition
- 4.3. Bibliographical notes
- 5 Determiners: articles and pronouns
- Introduction
- 5.1. Articles
- 5.2. Pronouns
- 5.3. Bibliographical notes
- 6 Numerals and quantifiers
- 7 Pre-determiners
- Introduction
- 7.1. The universal quantifier al 'all' and its alternants
- 7.2. The pre-determiner heel 'all/whole'
- 7.3. A note on focus particles
- 7.4. Bibliographical notes
- 8 Syntactic uses of noun phrases
- Adjectives and Adjective Phrases
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- 2 Projection of adjective phrases I: Complementation
- 3 Projection of adjective phrases II: Modification
- 4 Projection of adjective phrases III: Comparison
- 5 Attributive use of the adjective phrase
- 6 Predicative use of the adjective phrase
- 7 The partitive genitive construction
- 8 Adverbial use of the adjective phrase
- 9 Participles and infinitives: their adjectival use
- 10 Special constructions
- Adpositions and adpositional phrases
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- Introduction
- 1.1. Characterization of the category adposition
- 1.2. A formal classification of adpositional phrases
- 1.3. A semantic classification of adpositional phrases
- 1.3.1. Spatial adpositions
- 1.3.2. Temporal adpositions
- 1.3.3. Non-spatial/temporal prepositions
- 1.4. Borderline cases
- 1.5. Bibliographical notes
- 2 Projection of adpositional phrases: Complementation
- 3 Projection of adpositional phrases: Modification
- 4 Syntactic uses of the adpositional phrase
- 5 R-pronominalization and R-words
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- Phonology
-
- General
- Phonology
- Segment inventory
- Phonotactics
- Phonological Processes
- Assimilation
- Vowel nasalization
- Syllabic sonorants
- Final devoicing
- Fake geminates
- Vowel hiatus resolution
- Vowel reduction introduction
- Schwa deletion
- Schwa insertion
- /r/-deletion
- d-insertion
- {s/z}-insertion
- t-deletion
- Intrusive stop formation
- Breaking
- Vowel shortening
- h-deletion
- Replacement of the glide w
- Word stress
- Clitics
- Allomorphy
- Orthography of Frisian
- Morphology
- Inflection
- Word formation
- Derivation
- Prefixation
- Infixation
- Suffixation
- Nominal suffixes
- Verbal suffixes
- Adjectival suffixes
- Adverbial suffixes
- Numeral suffixes
- Interjectional suffixes
- Onomastic suffixes
- Conversion
- Compositions
- Derivation
- Syntax
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Unergative and unaccusative subjects
- Evidentiality
- To-infinitival clauses
- Predication and noun incorporation
- Ellipsis
- Imperativus-pro-Infinitivo
- Expression of irrealis
- Embedded Verb Second
- Agreement
- Negation
- Nouns & Noun Phrases
- Classification
- Complementation
- Modification
- Partitive noun constructions
- Referential partitive constructions
- Partitive measure nouns
- Numeral partitive constructions
- Partitive question constructions
- Nominalised quantifiers
- Kind partitives
- Partitive predication with prepositions
- Bare nominal attributions
- Articles and names
- Pronouns
- Quantifiers and (pre)determiners
- Interrogative pronouns
- R-pronouns
- Syntactic uses
- Adjective Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Complementation
- Modification and degree quantification
- Comparison by degree
- Comparative
- Superlative
- Equative
- Attribution
- Agreement
- Attributive adjectives vs. prenominal elements
- Complex adjectives
- Noun ellipsis
- Co-occurring adjectives
- Predication
- Partitive adjective constructions
- Adverbial use
- Participles and infinitives
- Adposition Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Complementation
- Modification
- Intransitive adpositions
- Predication
- Preposition stranding
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
-
- General
- Morphology
- Morphology
- 1 Word formation
- 1.1 Compounding
- 1.1.1 Compounds and their heads
- 1.1.2 Special types of compounds
- 1.1.2.1 Affixoids
- 1.1.2.2 Coordinative compounds
- 1.1.2.3 Synthetic compounds and complex pseudo-participles
- 1.1.2.4 Reduplicative compounds
- 1.1.2.5 Phrase-based compounds
- 1.1.2.6 Elative compounds
- 1.1.2.7 Exocentric compounds
- 1.1.2.8 Linking elements
- 1.1.2.9 Separable Complex Verbs and Particle Verbs
- 1.1.2.10 Noun Incorporation Verbs
- 1.1.2.11 Gapping
- 1.2 Derivation
- 1.3 Minor patterns of word formation
- 1.1 Compounding
- 2 Inflection
- 1 Word formation
- Morphology
- Syntax
- Adjectives and adjective phrases (APs)
- 0 Introduction to the AP
- 1 Characteristics and classification of APs
- 2 Complementation of APs
- 3 Modification and degree quantification of APs
- 4 Comparison by comparative, superlative and equative
- 5 Attribution of APs
- 6 Predication of APs
- 7 The partitive adjective construction
- 8 Adverbial use of APs
- 9 Participles and infinitives as APs
- Nouns and Noun Phrases (NPs)
- 0 Introduction to the NP
- 1 Characteristics and Classification of NPs
- 2 Complementation of NPs
- 3 Modification of NPs
- 3.1 Modification of NP by Determiners and APs
- 3.2 Modification of NP by PP
- 3.3 Modification of NP by adverbial clauses
- 3.4 Modification of NP by possessors
- 3.5 Modification of NP by relative clauses
- 3.6 Modification of NP in a cleft construction
- 3.7 Free relative clauses and selected interrogative clauses
- 4 Partitive noun constructions and constructions related to them
- 4.1 The referential partitive construction
- 4.2 The partitive construction of abstract quantity
- 4.3 The numerical partitive construction
- 4.4 The partitive interrogative construction
- 4.5 Adjectival, nominal and nominalised partitive quantifiers
- 4.6 Kind partitives
- 4.7 Partitive predication with a preposition
- 4.8 Bare nominal attribution
- 5 Articles and names
- 6 Pronouns
- 7 Quantifiers, determiners and predeterminers
- 8 Interrogative pronouns
- 9 R-pronouns and the indefinite expletive
- 10 Syntactic functions of Noun Phrases
- Adpositions and Adpositional Phrases (PPs)
- 0 Introduction to the PP
- 1 Characteristics and classification of PPs
- 2 Complementation of PPs
- 3 Modification of PPs
- 4 Bare (intransitive) adpositions
- 5 Predication of PPs
- 6 Form and distribution of adpositions with respect to staticity and construction type
- 7 Adpositional complements and adverbials
- Verbs and Verb Phrases (VPs)
- 0 Introduction to the VP in Saterland Frisian
- 1 Characteristics and classification of verbs
- 2 Unergative and unaccusative subjects and the auxiliary of the perfect
- 3 Evidentiality in relation to perception and epistemicity
- 4 Types of to-infinitival constituents
- 5 Predication
- 5.1 The auxiliary of being and its selection restrictions
- 5.2 The auxiliary of going and its selection restrictions
- 5.3 The auxiliary of continuation and its selection restrictions
- 5.4 The auxiliary of coming and its selection restrictions
- 5.5 Modal auxiliaries and their selection restrictions
- 5.6 Auxiliaries of body posture and aspect and their selection restrictions
- 5.7 Transitive verbs of predication
- 5.8 The auxiliary of doing used as a semantically empty finite auxiliary
- 5.9 Supplementive predication
- 6 The verbal paradigm, irregularity and suppletion
- 7 Verb Second and the word order in main and embedded clauses
- 8 Various aspects of clause structure
- Adjectives and adjective phrases (APs)
-
- General
- Phonology
- Afrikaans phonology
- Segment inventory
- Overview of Afrikaans vowels
- The diphthongised long vowels /e/, /ø/ and /o/
- The unrounded mid-front vowel /ɛ/
- The unrounded low-central vowel /ɑ/
- The unrounded low-central vowel /a/
- The rounded mid-high back vowel /ɔ/
- The rounded high back vowel /u/
- The rounded and unrounded high front vowels /i/ and /y/
- The unrounded and rounded central vowels /ə/ and /œ/
- The diphthongs /əi/, /œy/ and /œu/
- Overview of Afrikaans consonants
- The bilabial plosives /p/ and /b/
- The alveolar plosives /t/ and /d/
- The velar plosives /k/ and /g/
- The bilabial nasal /m/
- The alveolar nasal /n/
- The velar nasal /ŋ/
- The trill /r/
- The lateral liquid /l/
- The alveolar fricative /s/
- The velar fricative /x/
- The labiodental fricatives /f/ and /v/
- The approximants /ɦ/, /j/ and /ʋ/
- Overview of Afrikaans vowels
- Word stress
- The phonetic properties of stress
- Primary stress on monomorphemic words in Afrikaans
- Background to primary stress in monomorphemes in Afrikaans
- Overview of the Main Stress Rule of Afrikaans
- The short vowels of Afrikaans
- Long vowels in monomorphemes
- Primary stress on diphthongs in monomorphemes
- Exceptions
- Stress shifts in place names
- Stress shift towards word-final position
- Stress pattern of reduplications
- Phonological processes
- Vowel related processes
- Consonant related processes
- Homorganic glide insertion
- Phonology-morphology interface
- Phonotactics
- Morphology
- Syntax
- Afrikaans syntax
- Nouns and noun phrases
- Characteristics of the NP
- Classification of nouns
- Complementation of NPs
- Modification of NPs
- Binominal and partitive constructions
- Referential partitive constructions
- Partitive measure nouns
- Numeral partitive constructions
- Partitive question constructions
- Partitive constructions with nominalised quantifiers
- Partitive predication with prepositions
- Binominal name constructions
- Binominal genitive constructions
- Bare nominal attribution
- Articles and names
- Pronouns
- Quantifiers, determiners and predeterminers
- Syntactic uses of the noun phrase
- Adjectives and adjective phrases
- Characteristics and classification of the AP
- Complementation of APs
- Modification and Degree Quantification of APs
- Comparison by comparative, superlative and equative degree
- Attribution of APs
- Predication of APs
- The partitive adjective construction
- Adverbial use of APs
- Participles and infinitives as adjectives
- Verbs and verb phrases
- Characterisation and classification
- Argument structure
- Verb frame alternations
- Complements of non-main verbs
- Verb clusters
- Complement clauses
- Adverbial modification
- Word order in the clause: Introduction
- Word order in the clause: position of the finite Verb
- Word order in the clause: Clause-initial position
- Word order in the clause: Extraposition and right-dislocation in the postverbal field
- Word order in the middle field
- Emphatic constructions
- Adpositions and adposition phrases
Abstract nouns denote entities that have a mental existence only, and therefore do not have any physical properties. Obvious examples are nouns like betekenis'meaning', liefde'love' and geloof'belief'. The set of abstract nouns also include nouns like verwoesting'destruction'; these nouns denote events, which are not directly perceivable and have no objective existence in the physical world, but are rather mental constructs built on the basis of observations relating to the participants and the results of the events in question. Nouns like aanname'assumption' or verzoek'request' are also taken to be abstract nouns; they denote mental constructs that are used to refer to propositional contents. The same thing holds for nouns like grootte'size' or schoonheid'beauty' that, instead of denoting concrete objects, denote properties of these objects.
Most traditional (and also many theory-specific) discussions of abstract nouns treat these nouns as belonging to one heterogeneous group, their common feature being that they are not concrete. This is hardly surprising, since the category seems to defy systematic classification in terms of clear-cut features comparable to ±shape and ±set, which were used in 1.2.2.1 for the classification of the concrete nouns. Nevertheless, attempts have been made to come to a subclassification depending on the type of abstract entity denoted. Table 6 gives an overview of the types of abstract nouns that will be distinguished here together with a number of examples.
type of noun | denotation | examples |
State-of-affairs nouns | action +contr+dyn | verwoesting‘destruction’ behandeling‘treatment’ |
process -contr+dyn | val‘fall’ vooruitgang‘progress’ | |
position +contr-dyn | verblijf‘stay’ volharding‘perseverance’ | |
state -contr-dyn | bewusteloosheid‘unconsciousness’ ligging‘position’ | |
Proposition nouns | Propositional content | feit‘fact’ aanname‘assumption’ |
Speech-act nouns | statement | verklaring‘statement’, belofte‘promise’ |
question | vraag‘question’, verzoek‘request’ | |
order | bevel‘order’, opdracht‘order’ | |
Property nouns | Physical property | lengte‘length’, schoonheid‘beauty’ |
Mental property | geduld‘patience’, verlegenheid‘shyness’ | |
Emotion nouns | Emotion | liefde‘love’, angst‘fear’ |
In what follows, these types will be described in some detail. A brief discussion of differences in syntactic and morphological behavior between these subcategories will also be included.
State-of-affairs nouns, which are sometimes also referred to as event or process nouns, can be used to refer to states of affairs, that is, to actions (like verwoesting'destruction'), processes (like val'fall'), positions (like verblijf'stay'), states (like bewusteloosheid'unconsciousness'), and the like. As these states of affairs take place or obtain at a particular time and place, state-of-affairs nouns can be modified by time or place adverbials. Similarly, as states of affairs have participants, these nouns have arguments like agents, themes and recipients, even if these arguments are often not overtly present.
Let us consider some examples. The head noun verwoesting'destruction' in (75a) is used to denote an action, the noun val'fall' in (75b) denotes a process, the noun wonen'living' in (75c) denotes a position and the noun hebben'having' in (75d) denotes a state. In all these examples, the head noun is complemented by one or more noun phrases referring to the participant(s) in the state of affairs referred to by the noun phrase as a whole; see Section 2.2.3 for a more detailed discussion of these complements. Note that in all these examples the state-of-affairs nouns are typically derived from verbs; in view of their meaning, this will not come as a surprise.
a. | de verwoesting | van de stad | action: [+dynamic][+controlled] | |
the destruction | of the city |
b. | de val | van de regering | process: [+dynamic][-controlled] | |
the fall | of the government |
c. | het wonen | in een stad | position: [-dynamic][+controlled] | |
the living | in a city | |||
'living in a city' |
d. | het hebben | van blauwe ogen | state: [-dynamic][-controlled] | |
the having | of blue eyes | |||
'having blue eyes' |
Proposition nouns are sometimes also referred to as content nouns; they denote entities that are assumed to have contents, such as facts, ideas, assumptions, beliefs etc. These entities function as propositions in the sense that they can be given a truth-value: they can be said to be true or untrue, they can be believed or denied, agreed with or rejected etc. The nouns in question have a complement, which typically takes the form of a clause or a PP: in (76a-c) the noun is complemented by a dat-clause denoting the content of the proposition and in (76d) a prepositional van-phrase is used.
a. | het feit | dat | de aarde | rond | is | |
the fact | that | the earth | round | is | ||
'the fact that the earth is round' |
b. | het idee | dat | schapen | gekloond | kunnen | worden | |
the idea | that | sheep | cloned | can | be | ||
'the idea that sheep can be cloned' |
c. | de aanname | dat | er | leven | op de maan | is | |
the assumption | that | there | life | on the moon | is | ||
'the assumption that there is life on the moon' |
d. | het nieuws | van het aanstaande huwelijk van de kroonprins | |
the news | of the forthcoming marriage of the crown.prince | ||
'the news of the crown princeʼs forthcoming marriage' |
Like state-of-affairs nouns, proposition nouns are typically, though not necessarily, deverbal; cf. the use of the simple nouns feit'fact' and idee'idea' in (76a&b). With the deverbal cases, the input verb belongs to the class of so-called verbs of thinking (such as geloven'to believe', aannemen'to assume', veronderstellen'to suppose', denken'to think', menen'to think/believe', argumenteren'to argue/reason', redeneren'to reason', etc.) or verbs denoting actions requiring some mental activity on the part of the speaker or hearer (like impliceren'to imply', bewijzen'to prove').
The primed examples in (77) show that deverbal proposition nouns all seem to have infinitival counterparts that are clearly related in meaning.
a. | de aanname | |
'the assumption' |
a'. | het aannemen | |
'the assuming' |
b. | de argumentatie | |
'the argumentation' |
b'. | het argumenteren | |
'the arguing' |
c. | de redenering | |
'the reasoning/argumentation' |
c'. | het redeneren | |
'the reasoning' |
Nevertheless, there are important semantic and syntactic differences between the two forms. As far as the semantics is concerned, deverbal proposition nouns like aanname, argumentatie, and redenering denote the content of the argumentation or (line of) reasoning, whereas the infinitival nouns function as state-of-affairs nouns, denoting the action of arguing or reasoning. In other words, while the former are preferred in contexts like (78a) where it is the content that is referred to, the latter are more acceptable in contexts like (78b) where some action is referred to.
a. | Zijn redenering | was niet | bepaald | logisch. | |
his reasoning | was not | exactly | logical |
a'. | ?? | Zijn | redeneren | was niet | bepaald | logisch. |
his | to.reason | was not | exactly | logical |
b. | Logisch | redeneren | is niet | zijn sterkste punt. | |
logically | to.reason | is not | his strongest point | ||
'Reasoning logically isnʼt his strongest point.' |
b'. | ?? | Logische | redenering | is niet | zijn sterkste punt. |
logical | reasoning | is not | his strongest point |
We refer the reader to Section 2.2.3.2 for a more detailed discussion of these forms and the syntactic differences between them (such as the optional/obligatory realization of the complement).
Speech-act nouns denote a type of abstract entity that can be described as a speech act. Nouns of this type, such as vraag'question', bevel'order', belofte'promise', verzoek'request', mededeling'announcement' denote some form of verbal interaction, and are typically derived from verbs denoting such activities, that is, the input verb is a verb of saying like vragen'to ask', bevelen'to order', beloven'to promise', verzoeken'to request', etc.
Like proposition nouns, speech-act nouns can take a clausal complement introduced by a complementizer denoting the contents of the speech act. This is illustrated in example (79) for the speech-act nouns mededeling'announcement' and verzekering'assurance', which take a clausal complement introduced by the complementizer dat'that'.
a. | De mededeling | dat de trein vertraagd was, | was niet | te verstaan. | |
the announcement | that the train delayed was | was not | to hear | ||
'The announcement that the train was delayed couldnʼt be heard.' |
b. | De verzekering | dat het probleem niet ernstig was, | stelde | ons | gerust. | |
the assurance | that the problem not serious was | put | us | at.ease | ||
'The assurance that the problem wasnʼt serious put our minds at ease.' |
Speech-act nouns can also take an infinitival complement introduced by the complementizer om, provided the input verb is able to do so, too. The implied subject PRO of the infinitival complement clause is interpreted as being coreferential with an argument of the speech-act noun. Which argument functions as antecedent depends on the context, just as in the case of verbs; cf. Section V5.2.2.1 for extensive discussion. The examples in (80) serve to illustrate this: in (80a), it is the genitive noun phrase Jans, referring to the person making the request, that will be interpreted as coreferential with PRO, and in (80b), it is the noun phrase Peter, the recipient of the request, that is interpreted as the PRO subject of the interpretation.
a. | Jans verzoek aan Peter | [om PRO | te mogen | blijven] | werd | genegeerd. | |
Janʼs request to Peter | comp | to be.allowed | stay | was | ignored | ||
'Janʼs request to Peter for permission to stay was ignored.' |
b. | Jans verzoek aan Peter | [om PRO | te blijven] | werd | genegeerd. | |
Janʼs request to Peter | comp | to stay | was | ignored | ||
'Janʼs request to Peter to stay was ignored.' |
The clausal complement of the speech-act noun vraag'question' is interrogative. As with the verb vragen'to ask', the interrogative complement can be a yes/no-question, introduced by the complementizer of, or a wh-question, introduced by a wh-phrase.
a. | de vraag | [of | we | moesten | komen] | yes/no-question | |
the question | comp | we | must | come | |||
'the question as to whether we had to come' |
b. | de vraag | [hoe | we | nu | moesten | handelen] | wh-question | |
the question | how | we | now | must | act | |||
'the question as to how we should act in such cases' |
Speech-act nouns can also take PP-complements. These complements can denote the contents of the speech act, in which case the choice of preposition often depends on the speech-act noun.
a. | zijn verzoek | om salarisverhoging | |
his request | for pay rise |
b. | zijn mededeling | over het volgende uitje | |
'his announcement about the next excursion' |
c. | het verbod | op roken | in dit gebouw | |
the ban | on smoking | in this building |
d. | de vraag | naar olie | |
the request | for oil |
A postnominal PP can, of course, also refer to the participants of the speech act, in which case the prepositions van'of' and aan'to' are used, followed by a noun phrase referring to the speaker and the addressee, respectively, as in (83). Note that both the van-PP and the aan-PP precede the om-complement in (83), regardless of whether the latter is a PP or a clause.
a. | het verzoek | van Marie | aan de commissie | om extra hulp | |
the request | of Marie | to the committee | for extra help |
b. | het verzoek | van Marie | aan de commissie | [om PRO | te worden | toegelaten] | |
the request | of Marie | to the committee | comp | to be | admitted |
The examples in (84) show that a postnominal PP can also have the function of an adverbial adjunct. These examples also show that a PP-complement must precede the PP-adjunct, whereas a complement clause follows it instead. For more details on the complementation of speech-act nouns, see Section 2.3.
a. | het verbod | <op stelen> | in de Bijbel <??op stelen> | |
the ban | on stealing | in the Bible |
b. | het verbod | <??[om PRO | te stelen]> | in de Bijbel <[om PRO te stelen]> | |
the ban | comp | to steal | in the Bible |
Property nouns are those nouns that denote properties of entities. Two basic subtypes can be distinguished: (i) nouns describing physical/perceptible properties of concrete entities, such as hoogte'height', grootte'size', vorm'form', etc. and (ii) nouns describing abstract properties, such as character traits, like geduld'patience' or beleefdheid'politeness'. Property nouns, if derived, typically have an adjectival basis, such as hoog'high', breed'wide', groot'big', beleefd'polite', etc. Some basic property nouns, such as duur'duration' and kleur'color' have a verbal counterpart (duren'to last' and kleuren'to color'), but whether these nouns have been derived from the verbs in question or the other way round is an open question.
Since the physical properties denoted by property nouns are typically used to describe some other entity, they usually occur with a van-complement, as shown by examples (85a&b). Example (85c) shows that it is sometimes also possible to use a possessive pronoun or a genitive noun phrase.
a. | de hoogte van de toren | is indrukwekkend | |
the height of the tower | is impressive |
b. | De vorm van de vraag | is belangrijk. | |
the form of the question | is important |
c. | de lengte van Jan/Jans lengte/zijn lengte | |
the height of Jan/Janʼs height/his height |
Genitive noun phrases can only be used if the referent is +human, as in (85c), but possessive pronouns can also be used to refer to a -animate entity. This is, however, subject to certain restrictions that are not fully understood, and using a pronominalized van-PP is often preferred; see Section 5.2.2.1 for detailed discussion. Example (86c) shows that using a pronominalized van-PP to refer to a +human referent gives rise to a degraded result.
a. | De hoogte ervan/$Zijn hoogte | is indrukwekkend. | |
the height of.it/his height | is impressive | ||
'Its height is impressive.' |
b. | De vorm ervan/$Zijn vorm | is belangrijk. | |
the form of.it/his form | is important | ||
'Its form is important.' |
c. | Zijn[+human] lengte/??de lengte ervan[+human] | |
his height/the height of him |
A van-complement is not used if the reference is nonspecific or generic: in the former case the property noun will be preceded by the indefinite article, as in (87a), and in the latter case it may appear without a determiner, as in (87b).
a. | Elk gebouw | heeft | een hoogte, | een lengte | en | een breedte. | |
every building | has | a height | a length | and | a width |
b. | Vorm | is belangrijker | dan inhoud. | |
form | is more important | than content | ||
'Form is more important than content.' |
The second subcategory of property nouns consists of nouns that denote properties that cannot be observed or measured in a direct way, but which form part of the mental make-up of the entity described. They include nouns denoting (more or less) permanent character traits like geduld'patience', intelligentie'intelligence' or luiheid'laziness'. As with the physical property nouns, these nouns typically occur in combination with a van-PP or, when the property is assigned to a +human entity, with a genitive noun phrase or possessive pronoun. This is illustrated in example (88).
a. | de aantrekkingskracht | van drugs | |
the attraction | of drugs |
b. | het geduld | van Peter | |
the patience | of Peter |
b'. | Peters/zijn geduld | |
Peterʼs/his patience |
The emotion nouns are the final type of abstract nouns that we will discuss here, These nouns denote (more or less) temporary emotions, like haat'hatred', begeerte'desire', behoefte'need' and verdriet'grief'. In most cases, the emotion denoted involves some other, affected, entity. In this respect, the nouns exhibit a structural parallelism with the verbs to which they are semantically related; this will become clear from comparing the primeless, verbal constructions in (89) with the primed, nominal ones. However, the fact that these nouns select their own preposition (voor, aan, naar, tegen/jegens), which is typically lacking in the verbal constructions, suggests that these nouns cannot be considered derived from the related verbs. See Section 2.1, sub VA5, for some more discussion on the emotion nouns.
a. | Peter | behoeft | rust. | archaic | |
Peter | needs | quiet |
a'. | Peters | behoefte | aan rust | colloquial | |
Peterʼs | need | for quiet |
b. | Zij | begeert | macht. | |
she | craves | power |
b'. | haar | begeerte | naar macht | |
her | craving | for power |
c. | Hij | haat | zijn rivaal. | |
he | hates | his rival |
c'. | zijn haat | tegen/jegens zijn rivaal | |
his hatred | of his rival |
In many cases nouns that can be used to refer to abstract entities can also be used to refer to concrete entities. This type of ambiguity has often been referred to as the difference between state-of-affairs nouns and result nouns, with the former denoting an event and the latter the concrete result of that event; cf. Abney (1987: 115), Grimshaw (1990), De Haas & Trommelen (1993: 241) and Alexiadou et al. (2007: part IV, Section 1.3). Examples of such nouns are uitvinding'invention' and bestrating'surfacing/surface' in example (90). Observe the difference in complementation between the two (a)-examples in (90), with the van-PP referring to the invention and the inventor, respectively; see Section 2.2.3.3 for more details on the complementation of ing-nominalizations.
a. | De uitvinding van de telefoon | dateert | uit de 19e eeuw. | process noun | |
the invention of the telephone | dates | from the 19th century |
a'. | De uitvinding van Bell | hing | aan de muur. | result noun | |
the invention of Bell | hangs | on the wall | |||
'Bellʼs invention is hanging on the wall.' |
b. | De bestrating van de weg | duurde | drie weken. | process noun | |
the surfacing of the road | took | three weeks |
b'. | De bestrating van deze weg | moet | vernieuwd | worden. | result noun | |
the surface of this road | needs | renewed | be | |||
'This road is in need of a new surface.' |
The distinction between result and process nouns covers only a small number of the many ambiguities that may occur with abstract nouns. Many nouns can be used to denote an abstract entity like an action or process as well as a concrete entity that is not the result of the state of affairs but in some other way related to it. In examples (91a'&b'), for instance, the nouns vergadering and bezoek refer to participants (namely, the agents) of the action denoted by the verbs vergaderen'to meet' and bezoeken'to visit', and in (91c') the noun huisvesting'housing' refers to the (concrete) means through which the action of housing is accomplished. In all three cases the nouns in the primeless sentences are state-of-affairs nouns.
a. | De vergadering | duurde | drie uur. | process noun | |
the meeting | lasted | three hour |
a'. | De vergadering | bestond | uit oudere heren. | “participant” noun | |
the meeting | consisted | of elderly gentlemen |
b. | Het bezoek | duurde | erg lang. | process noun | |
the visit | lasted | very long |
b'. | Het bezoek | bleef | erg lang. | “participant” noun | |
the visitors | stayed | very long |
c. | De huisvesting van asielzoekers | duurt | te lang. | process noun | |
the housing of asylum seekers | takes | too long |
c'. | We | waren | op zoek | naar geschikte huisvesting. | “means” noun | |
we | were | looking | for suitable housing | |||
'We were looking for suitable accommodation.' |
In other cases, the abstract noun in question does have an event and a result reading, but instead of the result being a concrete entity, its referent, too, is abstract. An example is given in (92): in (92a) the noun veroordeling is used to refer to the action of sentencing performed by the jury, whereas in (92b) it is used to refer to the punishment resulting from this action.
a. | De veroordeling van de beklaagde | door de jury | verliep | moeizaam. | |
the sentencing of the accused | by the jury | went | difficult | ||
'The sentencing of the accused by the jury was problematic.' |
b. | De verdachte | wachtte | een zware veroordeling. | |
the accused | waited | a heavy sentence | ||
'The accused was in for a heavy sentence.' |
Finally, example (93) shows that the abstract noun need not be a state-of-affairs noun. Instead the ambiguity here is between an abstract, speech act reading of the nouns vraag'question' and bevel'order' and a concrete reading. In these cases, the speech-act reading is clearly the prototypical one.
a. | Hij | had | de vraag/het bevel | niet | goed | begrepen. | abstract | |
he | had | the question/the order | not | well | understood | |||
'He hadnʼt quite understood the question/order.' |
b. | De vraag/het bevel | was moeilijk | te lezen. | concrete | |
the question/the order | was difficult | to read |
- 1987The English noun phrase in its sentential aspectCambridge,, MAMITThesis
- 2007Noun phrases in the generative perspectiveBerlin/New YorkMouton de Gruyter
- 1990Argument structureLI Monograph 18Cambridge, MAMIT Press
- 1993Morfologisch handboek van het Nederlands. Een overzicht van de woordvormingSDU Uitgeverij