- Dutch
- Frisian
- Saterfrisian
- Afrikaans
-
- Phonology
- Segment inventory
- Phonotactics
- Phonological processes
- Phonology-morphology interface
- Word stress
- Primary stress in simplex words
- Monomorphemic words
- Diachronic aspects
- Generalizations on stress placement
- Default penultimate stress
- Lexical stress
- The closed penult restriction
- Final closed syllables
- The diphthong restriction
- Superheavy syllables (SHS)
- The three-syllable window
- Segmental restrictions
- Phonetic correlates
- Stress shifts in loanwords
- Quantity-sensitivity
- Secondary stress
- Vowel reduction in unstressed syllables
- Stress in complex words
- Primary stress in simplex words
- Accent & intonation
- Clitics
- Spelling
- Morphology
- Word formation
- Compounding
- Nominal compounds
- Verbal compounds
- Adjectival compounds
- Affixoids
- Coordinative compounds
- Synthetic compounds
- Reduplicative compounds
- Phrase-based compounds
- Elative compounds
- Exocentric compounds
- Linking elements
- Separable complex verbs (SCVs)
- Gapping of complex words
- Particle verbs
- Copulative compounds
- Derivation
- Numerals
- Derivation: inputs and input restrictions
- The meaning of affixes
- Non-native morphology
- Cohering and non-cohering affixes
- Prefixation
- Suffixation
- Nominal suffixation: person nouns
- Conversion
- Pseudo-participles
- Bound forms
- Nouns
- Nominal prefixes
- Nominal suffixes
- -aal and -eel
- -aar
- -aard
- -aat
- -air
- -aris
- -ast
- Diminutives
- -dom
- -een
- -ees
- -el (nominal)
- -elaar
- -enis
- -er (nominal)
- -erd
- -erik
- -es
- -eur
- -euse
- ge...te
- -heid
- -iaan, -aan
- -ief
- -iek
- -ier
- -ier (French)
- -ière
- -iet
- -igheid
- -ij and allomorphs
- -ijn
- -in
- -ing
- -isme
- -ist
- -iteit
- -ling
- -oir
- -oot
- -rice
- -schap
- -schap (de)
- -schap (het)
- -sel
- -st
- -ster
- -t
- -tal
- -te
- -voud
- Verbs
- Adjectives
- Adverbs
- Univerbation
- Neo-classical word formation
- Construction-dependent morphology
- Morphological productivity
- Compounding
- Inflection
- Inflection and derivation
- Allomorphy
- The interface between phonology and morphology
- Word formation
- Syntax
- Preface and acknowledgements
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
- 1 Characterization and classification
- 2 Projection of verb phrases I:Argument structure
- 3 Projection of verb phrases II:Verb frame alternations
- Introduction
- 3.1. Main types
- 3.2. Alternations involving the external argument
- 3.3. Alternations of noun phrases and PPs
- 3.3.1. Dative/PP alternations (dative shift)
- 3.3.1.1. Dative alternation with aan-phrases (recipients)
- 3.3.1.2. Dative alternation with naar-phrases (goals)
- 3.3.1.3. Dative alternation with van-phrases (sources)
- 3.3.1.4. Dative alternation with bij-phrases (possessors)
- 3.3.1.5. Dative alternation with voor-phrases (benefactives)
- 3.3.1.6. Conclusion
- 3.3.1.7. Bibliographical notes
- 3.3.2. Accusative/PP alternations
- 3.3.3. Nominative/PP alternations
- 3.3.1. Dative/PP alternations (dative shift)
- 3.4. Some apparent cases of verb frame alternation
- 3.5. Bibliographical notes
- 4 Projection of verb phrases IIIa:Selection of clauses/verb phrases
- 5 Projection of verb phrases IIIb:Argument and complementive clauses
- Introduction
- 5.1. Finite argument clauses
- 5.2. Infinitival argument clauses
- 5.3. Complementive clauses
- 6 Projection of verb phrases IIIc:Complements of non-main verbs
- 7 Projection of verb phrases IIId:Verb clusters
- 8 Projection of verb phrases IV: Adverbial modification
- 9 Word order in the clause I:General introduction
- 10 Word order in the clause II:Position of the finite verb (verb-first/second)
- 11 Word order in the clause III:Clause-initial position (wh-movement)
- Introduction
- 11.1. The formation of V1- and V2-clauses
- 11.2. Clause-initial position remains (phonetically) empty
- 11.3. Clause-initial position is filled
- 12 Word order in the clause IV:Postverbal field (extraposition)
- 13 Word order in the clause V: Middle field (scrambling)
- 14 Main-clause external elements
- Nouns and Noun Phrases
- 1 Characterization and classification
- 2 Projection of noun phrases I: complementation
- Introduction
- 2.1. General observations
- 2.2. Prepositional and nominal complements
- 2.3. Clausal complements
- 2.4. Bibliographical notes
- 3 Projection of noun phrases II: modification
- Introduction
- 3.1. Restrictive and non-restrictive modifiers
- 3.2. Premodification
- 3.3. Postmodification
- 3.3.1. Adpositional phrases
- 3.3.2. Relative clauses
- 3.3.3. Infinitival clauses
- 3.3.4. A special case: clauses referring to a proposition
- 3.3.5. Adjectival phrases
- 3.3.6. Adverbial postmodification
- 3.4. Bibliographical notes
- 4 Projection of noun phrases III: binominal constructions
- Introduction
- 4.1. Binominal constructions without a preposition
- 4.2. Binominal constructions with a preposition
- 4.3. Bibliographical notes
- 5 Determiners: articles and pronouns
- Introduction
- 5.1. Articles
- 5.2. Pronouns
- 5.3. Bibliographical notes
- 6 Numerals and quantifiers
- 7 Pre-determiners
- Introduction
- 7.1. The universal quantifier al 'all' and its alternants
- 7.2. The pre-determiner heel 'all/whole'
- 7.3. A note on focus particles
- 7.4. Bibliographical notes
- 8 Syntactic uses of noun phrases
- Adjectives and Adjective Phrases
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- 2 Projection of adjective phrases I: Complementation
- 3 Projection of adjective phrases II: Modification
- 4 Projection of adjective phrases III: Comparison
- 5 Attributive use of the adjective phrase
- 6 Predicative use of the adjective phrase
- 7 The partitive genitive construction
- 8 Adverbial use of the adjective phrase
- 9 Participles and infinitives: their adjectival use
- 10 Special constructions
- Adpositions and adpositional phrases
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- Introduction
- 1.1. Characterization of the category adposition
- 1.2. A formal classification of adpositional phrases
- 1.3. A semantic classification of adpositional phrases
- 1.3.1. Spatial adpositions
- 1.3.2. Temporal adpositions
- 1.3.3. Non-spatial/temporal prepositions
- 1.4. Borderline cases
- 1.5. Bibliographical notes
- 2 Projection of adpositional phrases: Complementation
- 3 Projection of adpositional phrases: Modification
- 4 Syntactic uses of the adpositional phrase
- 5 R-pronominalization and R-words
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- Phonology
-
- General
- Phonology
- Segment inventory
- Phonotactics
- Phonological Processes
- Assimilation
- Vowel nasalization
- Syllabic sonorants
- Final devoicing
- Fake geminates
- Vowel hiatus resolution
- Vowel reduction introduction
- Schwa deletion
- Schwa insertion
- /r/-deletion
- d-insertion
- {s/z}-insertion
- t-deletion
- Intrusive stop formation
- Breaking
- Vowel shortening
- h-deletion
- Replacement of the glide w
- Word stress
- Clitics
- Allomorphy
- Orthography of Frisian
- Morphology
- Inflection
- Word formation
- Derivation
- Prefixation
- Infixation
- Suffixation
- Nominal suffixes
- Verbal suffixes
- Adjectival suffixes
- Adverbial suffixes
- Numeral suffixes
- Interjectional suffixes
- Onomastic suffixes
- Conversion
- Compositions
- Derivation
- Syntax
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Unergative and unaccusative subjects
- Evidentiality
- To-infinitival clauses
- Predication and noun incorporation
- Ellipsis
- Imperativus-pro-Infinitivo
- Expression of irrealis
- Embedded Verb Second
- Agreement
- Negation
- Nouns & Noun Phrases
- Classification
- Complementation
- Modification
- Partitive noun constructions
- Referential partitive constructions
- Partitive measure nouns
- Numeral partitive constructions
- Partitive question constructions
- Nominalised quantifiers
- Kind partitives
- Partitive predication with prepositions
- Bare nominal attributions
- Articles and names
- Pronouns
- Quantifiers and (pre)determiners
- Interrogative pronouns
- R-pronouns
- Syntactic uses
- Adjective Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Complementation
- Modification and degree quantification
- Comparison by degree
- Comparative
- Superlative
- Equative
- Attribution
- Agreement
- Attributive adjectives vs. prenominal elements
- Complex adjectives
- Noun ellipsis
- Co-occurring adjectives
- Predication
- Partitive adjective constructions
- Adverbial use
- Participles and infinitives
- Adposition Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Complementation
- Modification
- Intransitive adpositions
- Predication
- Preposition stranding
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
-
- General
- Morphology
- Morphology
- 1 Word formation
- 1.1 Compounding
- 1.1.1 Compounds and their heads
- 1.1.2 Special types of compounds
- 1.1.2.1 Affixoids
- 1.1.2.2 Coordinative compounds
- 1.1.2.3 Synthetic compounds and complex pseudo-participles
- 1.1.2.4 Reduplicative compounds
- 1.1.2.5 Phrase-based compounds
- 1.1.2.6 Elative compounds
- 1.1.2.7 Exocentric compounds
- 1.1.2.8 Linking elements
- 1.1.2.9 Separable Complex Verbs and Particle Verbs
- 1.1.2.10 Noun Incorporation Verbs
- 1.1.2.11 Gapping
- 1.2 Derivation
- 1.3 Minor patterns of word formation
- 1.1 Compounding
- 2 Inflection
- 1 Word formation
- Morphology
- Syntax
- Adjectives and adjective phrases (APs)
- 0 Introduction to the AP
- 1 Characteristics and classification of APs
- 2 Complementation of APs
- 3 Modification and degree quantification of APs
- 4 Comparison by comparative, superlative and equative
- 5 Attribution of APs
- 6 Predication of APs
- 7 The partitive adjective construction
- 8 Adverbial use of APs
- 9 Participles and infinitives as APs
- Nouns and Noun Phrases (NPs)
- 0 Introduction to the NP
- 1 Characteristics and Classification of NPs
- 2 Complementation of NPs
- 3 Modification of NPs
- 3.1 Modification of NP by Determiners and APs
- 3.2 Modification of NP by PP
- 3.3 Modification of NP by adverbial clauses
- 3.4 Modification of NP by possessors
- 3.5 Modification of NP by relative clauses
- 3.6 Modification of NP in a cleft construction
- 3.7 Free relative clauses and selected interrogative clauses
- 4 Partitive noun constructions and constructions related to them
- 4.1 The referential partitive construction
- 4.2 The partitive construction of abstract quantity
- 4.3 The numerical partitive construction
- 4.4 The partitive interrogative construction
- 4.5 Adjectival, nominal and nominalised partitive quantifiers
- 4.6 Kind partitives
- 4.7 Partitive predication with a preposition
- 4.8 Bare nominal attribution
- 5 Articles and names
- 6 Pronouns
- 7 Quantifiers, determiners and predeterminers
- 8 Interrogative pronouns
- 9 R-pronouns and the indefinite expletive
- 10 Syntactic functions of Noun Phrases
- Adpositions and Adpositional Phrases (PPs)
- 0 Introduction to the PP
- 1 Characteristics and classification of PPs
- 2 Complementation of PPs
- 3 Modification of PPs
- 4 Bare (intransitive) adpositions
- 5 Predication of PPs
- 6 Form and distribution of adpositions with respect to staticity and construction type
- 7 Adpositional complements and adverbials
- Verbs and Verb Phrases (VPs)
- 0 Introduction to the VP in Saterland Frisian
- 1 Characteristics and classification of verbs
- 2 Unergative and unaccusative subjects and the auxiliary of the perfect
- 3 Evidentiality in relation to perception and epistemicity
- 4 Types of to-infinitival constituents
- 5 Predication
- 5.1 The auxiliary of being and its selection restrictions
- 5.2 The auxiliary of going and its selection restrictions
- 5.3 The auxiliary of continuation and its selection restrictions
- 5.4 The auxiliary of coming and its selection restrictions
- 5.5 Modal auxiliaries and their selection restrictions
- 5.6 Auxiliaries of body posture and aspect and their selection restrictions
- 5.7 Transitive verbs of predication
- 5.8 The auxiliary of doing used as a semantically empty finite auxiliary
- 5.9 Supplementive predication
- 6 The verbal paradigm, irregularity and suppletion
- 7 Verb Second and the word order in main and embedded clauses
- 8 Various aspects of clause structure
- Adjectives and adjective phrases (APs)
-
- General
- Phonology
- Afrikaans phonology
- Segment inventory
- Overview of Afrikaans vowels
- The diphthongised long vowels /e/, /ø/ and /o/
- The unrounded mid-front vowel /ɛ/
- The unrounded low-central vowel /ɑ/
- The unrounded low-central vowel /a/
- The rounded mid-high back vowel /ɔ/
- The rounded high back vowel /u/
- The rounded and unrounded high front vowels /i/ and /y/
- The unrounded and rounded central vowels /ə/ and /œ/
- The diphthongs /əi/, /œy/ and /œu/
- Overview of Afrikaans consonants
- The bilabial plosives /p/ and /b/
- The alveolar plosives /t/ and /d/
- The velar plosives /k/ and /g/
- The bilabial nasal /m/
- The alveolar nasal /n/
- The velar nasal /ŋ/
- The trill /r/
- The lateral liquid /l/
- The alveolar fricative /s/
- The velar fricative /x/
- The labiodental fricatives /f/ and /v/
- The approximants /ɦ/, /j/ and /ʋ/
- Overview of Afrikaans vowels
- Word stress
- The phonetic properties of stress
- Primary stress on monomorphemic words in Afrikaans
- Background to primary stress in monomorphemes in Afrikaans
- Overview of the Main Stress Rule of Afrikaans
- The short vowels of Afrikaans
- Long vowels in monomorphemes
- Primary stress on diphthongs in monomorphemes
- Exceptions
- Stress shifts in place names
- Stress shift towards word-final position
- Stress pattern of reduplications
- Phonological processes
- Vowel related processes
- Consonant related processes
- Homorganic glide insertion
- Phonology-morphology interface
- Phonotactics
- Morphology
- Syntax
- Afrikaans syntax
- Nouns and noun phrases
- Characteristics of the NP
- Classification of nouns
- Complementation of NPs
- Modification of NPs
- Binominal and partitive constructions
- Referential partitive constructions
- Partitive measure nouns
- Numeral partitive constructions
- Partitive question constructions
- Partitive constructions with nominalised quantifiers
- Partitive predication with prepositions
- Binominal name constructions
- Binominal genitive constructions
- Bare nominal attribution
- Articles and names
- Pronouns
- Quantifiers, determiners and predeterminers
- Syntactic uses of the noun phrase
- Adjectives and adjective phrases
- Characteristics and classification of the AP
- Complementation of APs
- Modification and Degree Quantification of APs
- Comparison by comparative, superlative and equative degree
- Attribution of APs
- Predication of APs
- The partitive adjective construction
- Adverbial use of APs
- Participles and infinitives as adjectives
- Verbs and verb phrases
- Characterisation and classification
- Argument structure
- Verb frame alternations
- Complements of non-main verbs
- Verb clusters
- Complement clauses
- Adverbial modification
- Word order in the clause: Introduction
- Word order in the clause: position of the finite Verb
- Word order in the clause: Clause-initial position
- Word order in the clause: Extraposition and right-dislocation in the postverbal field
- Word order in the middle field
- Emphatic constructions
- Adpositions and adposition phrases
This section discusses the unmarked order of adverbial phrases. Establishing this order is not an easy task since the placement of adverbials exhibits a certain amount of freedom; adverbials are like most clausal constituents in that they may undergo various kinds of movement. Subsection I reviews a number of movement processes that may affect the surface order of adverbials, so as to restrict the discussion in such a way that we eliminate their interference as much as possible. Because it is relatively uncontroversial that VP adverbials follow clause adverbials in the unmarked order, we will be able to split our investigation into two parts: Subsections II and III discuss the unmarked word order of various subtypes of VP adverbials and clause adverbials, respectively. For want of sufficiently detailed research, the results in this section should be seen as preliminary, as will also be clear from the fact that we will have to leave various questions open for the moment.
This subsection will show that adverbials can undergo various kinds of movement, which complicates our investigation of the unmarked word order of adverbials considerably. We can curb the interference of movement, however, by investigating the order of adverbials in the middle field of the clause only, thus eliminating the effects of wh-movement and extraposition discussed in Subsections A and B. This reduction leaves us with movement operations affecting the word order in the middle field, such as focus movement and weak proform shift; these movement operations will be briefly addressed in Subsections C and D.
Most adverbials are like other clausal constituents in that they can be moved into clause-initial position under certain conditions. This holds especially for adverbial phrases that can be questioned, as shown in (180) for three kinds of VP adverbials.
a. | Hoe | heb | je | geslapen? | Erg goed! | manner | |
how | have | you | slept | very well | |||
'How did you sleep? Very well!' |
b. | Waarmee | heb | je | dat gat | gevuld? | Met zand. | means | |
with.what | have | you | that hole | filled | with sand | |||
'With what have you filled that hole? With sand.' |
c. | Met wie | heb | je | gedanst? | Met Jan. | comitative | |
with who | have | you | danced | with Jan | |||
'Who have you danced with? With Jan.' |
It will be clear that this sort of movement may affect the relative word order of adverbials when more than one adverbial phrase is present. This is illustrated in (181) for temporal and comitative adverbials; although we will see that there is reason for assuming that temporal adverbials precede comitative adverbials in the unmarked order, wh-movement can easily reverse this order.
a. | Jan heeft gisteren met Peter/ʼm gedanst. | |
Jan has yesterday with Peter/him danced | ||
'Jan danced with Peter/him yesterday.' |
b. | Met wie | heeft | Jan gisteren | gedanst? | |
with whom | has | Jan yesterday | danced | ||
'With whom did Jan dance yesterday?' |
Many adverbials that cannot be questioned can still be placed in sentence-initial position by topicalization. This holds, e.g., for modal adverbs; although Section 8.3.3 has shown that they cannot be questioned, the examples in (182) show that topicalization can change the unmarked order of the temporal clause adverbial morgen'tomorrow' and the adverb waarschijnlijk'probably'.
a. | Jan gaat | morgen | waarschijnlijk | al | om drie uur | weg. | |
Jan goes | tomorrow | probably | already | at 3 oʼclock | away | ||
'Jan will probably leave tomorrow at three o'clock already.' |
b. | Waarschijnlijk | gaat | Jan morgen | al | om drie uur | weg. | |
probably | goes | Jan tomorrow | already | at 3 oʼclock | away |
Note in passing that there are additional restrictions on wh-movement; the examples in (183) show, for instance, that while temporal VP adverbials may cross temporal clause adverbials in questions, this is more difficult in topicalization constructions. Since this kind of intervention effect has not been studied in detail, we leave the issue to future research.
a. | Hoe laat | gaat | Jan morgen | weg? | Om drie uur. | |
how late | goes | Jan tomorrow | away | at 3 oʼclock | ||
'When will Jan leave tomorrow? At 3 oʼclock.' |
b. | ?? | Om drie uur | gaat | Jan | morgen | weg. |
at 3 oʼclock | goes | Jan | tomorrow | away |
For our present purpose, it suffices to say that the effects of wh-movement can be easily eliminated by restricting our attention to the relative order of adverbials in the middle field of the clause; for a detailed discussion of wh-movement, we refer the reader to Section 11.3.
Another way of affecting the unmarked order of adverbials is by extraposition, which is especially common for adverbials of the category PP or clause. We will see later that there are grounds for assuming that contingency adverbials such as vanwege het mooie weer'because of the nice weather' in (184a) precede comitative adverbials such as met Els'with Els' in the unmarked order. Nevertheless, extraposition of the contingency adverbial can easily reverse this order, as shown in (184b). In fact, (184c) shows that simultaneous extraposition of the two adverbials also requires the order to be inverted, a phenomenon that has become known as the mirror effect; cf. Koster (1974).
a. | Jan is vanwege het mooie weer | met Els | gaan | wandelen. | |
Jan is because.of the nice weather | with Els | go | walk | ||
'Jan has gone walking with Els because of the nice weather.' |
b. | Jan is met Els | gaan | wandelen | vanwege het mooie weer. | |
Jan is with Els | go | walk | because.of the nice weather |
c. | Jan is gaan | wandelen | met Els | vanwege het mooie weer. | |
Jan is go | walk | with Els | because.of the nice weather |
For our present goal, it again suffices to say that the interference of extraposition can be easily eliminated by restricting our attention to the relative order of adverbials in the middle field of the clause; for a detailed discussion of extraposition including the mirror effect, we refer the reader to Chapter 12.
Even if we restrict our investigation to the middle field of the clause, we still have to deal with movement operations affecting the word order in this domain of the clause. One such movement operation is focus movement, which may move a contrastively focused phrase into a position preceding the negative clause adverb niet'not'. This is illustrated in (185), where focus accent is indicated by small caps.
a. | Jan heeft | niet met Marie gedanst. | |
Jan has | not with Marie danced | ||
'Jan hasnʼt danced with Marie.' |
b. | Jan heeft | met Marie | niet | gedanst | (maar | wel | met Els). | |
Jan has | with Marie | not | danced | but | aff | with Els | ||
'Jan hasnʼt danced with Marie (but he has with Els).' |
One way of excluding focus movement is by restricting our investigation to sentences with a neutral (non-contrastive) intonation pattern. With prepositional adverbial phrases it is often possible to exclude focus movement by using a weak pronoun as the complement of the preposition (or, alternatively, the weak pronominal PP ermee'with it'), as is illustrated in (186). For more information about focus movement, we refer the reader to Section 13.3.2.
Jan heeft | <*met ʼr> | niet <met ʼr> | gedanst. | ||
Jan has | with her | not | danced | ||
'Jan hasnʼt danced with her.' |
Weak proforms strongly prefer placement in the left periphery of the middle field of the clause, regardless of their syntactic function. That this also holds for adverbial phrases is illustrated by means of the locational adverbs in (187): while placing the adverbial PP in Leiden in a position preceding the modal adverb waarschijnlijk'probably' leads to a severely degraded result, the corresponding weak locational proform er must precede it.
a. | Jan woont | <*?in Leiden> | waarschijnlijk | al | jaren <in Leiden>. | |
Jan lives | in Leiden | probably | already | years | ||
'Jan has probably been living in Leiden for years.' |
b. | Jan woont | <er> | waarschijnlijk <*er> | al | jaren. | |
Jan lives | there | probably | already | years | ||
'Jan has probably lived there for years.' |
For our present purpose, it suffices to say that the effect of weak proform shift can be eliminated by simply excluding weak proforms from our investigation; for more discussion of weak proform shift, we refer the reader to Section 13.4.
This subsection has shown that the investigation of the unmarked order of adverbials is complicated by the fact that most adverbials are like other clausal constituents in that they can be moved under certain conditions. In order to eliminate the effects of movement as much as possible, we will restrict our investigation in the following subsections to the relative order of adverbials in the middle field of the clause. Furthermore, we will only discuss sentences with a neutral intonation pattern and avoid the use of weak adverbial proforms.
This subsection discusses the unmarked order of the VP adverbials in (188). Since Cinque’s (1999) seminal study on adverbial placement, it has often been claimed that the order of VP adverbials is essentially free. Schweikert (2005) and Cinque (2006) dismissed this claim, however, and argued that VP adverbials have a rigid underlying order. This section will show that this claim is indeed correct, although we will end up with somewhat different conclusions about the unmarked order of VP adverbials than the order proposed by Schweikert.
a. | Process: manner; instrument; means; volition; domain |
b. | Agentive: passive door-PP; comitative met-PP |
c. | Spatio-temporal: place; time |
d. | Contingency: cause, reason, purpose, result, concession |
e. | Predicate-degree: erg'very'; een beetje'a bit' |
We will investigate the unmarked order of the process adverbial by considering the placement of the various subtypes relative to adjectival manner adverbials such as zorgvuldig'carefully'. Although it is not difficult to find instrument/means adverbials to the left of manner adverbs, as illustrated in the primeless examples in (189), there is cause for assuming that this order is the result of focus movement: the primed examples show that their pronominalized counterpart ermee'with it' cannot precede the manner adverb but has to follow it.
a. | Jan heeft | de ring | <met een kwast> | zorgvuldig <met een kwast> | gereinigd. | |
Jan has | the ring | with a brush | carefully | cleaned | ||
'Jan has cleaned the ring carefully with a brush.' |
a'. | Jan heeft | de ring | <*ermee> | zorgvuldig <ermee> | gereinigd. | |
Jan has | the ring | with.it | carefully | cleaned | ||
'Jan has cleaned the ring carefully with it.' |
b. | Jan heeft | <met zand> | zorgvuldig | het gat <met zand> | gevuld. | |
Jan has | with sand | carefully | the hole | filled | ||
'Jan has filled the hole carefully with sand.' |
b'. | Jan heeft | <*ermee> | zorgvuldig | het gat <ermee> | gevuld. | |
Jan has | with.it | carefully | the hole | filled | ||
'Jan has filled the hole carefully with it.' |
Observe that pronominal PPs are preferably split, as in Jan heeft er de ring zorgvuldig mee gereinigd and Jan heeft er zorgvuldig het gat mee gevuld, but this is not relevant here. Since instrument and means adverbials do not easily co-occur, we will not discuss their relative order here.
Example (190a) shows that manner adverbs tend to precede domain adverbials under a non-contrastive intonation pattern: a Google search (11/3/2015) has shown that the order medisch–grondig/zorgvuldig occurred only 5 times, while the order grondig/zorgvuldig–medisch resulted in 50 hits. This finding is consistent with the fact that domain adverbials tend to follow instrumental PPs such as met medicijnen'with medicines' in example (190b). Recall that the judgments given only hold under a non-contrastive intonation pattern: assigning focus accent to medisch much improves the marked order.
a. | Jan is | <??medisch> | grondig/zorgvuldig <medisch> | onderzocht. | |
Jan has.been | medically | thoroughly/carefully | examined | ||
'Jan has been thoroughly/carefully examined medically.' |
b. | HIV | kan | <??medisch> | met medicijnen <medisch> | behandeld | worden. | |
HIV | can | medically | with medicines | treated | be | ||
'HIV can be medically treated with medicines.' |
Example (191a) finally shows that volition adverbials precede manner adverbs. By transitivity we can conclude that they will also precede the other process adverbials; that this conclusion is indeed correct is shown in (191b) for a means adverbial.
a. | dat | Jan zich | <vrijwillig> | intensief <*vrijwillig> | inzet | voor de club. | |
that | Jan refl | voluntarily | intensively | labors | for the club | ||
'that Jan voluntarily dedicates himself to the club intensively.' |
b. | dat | Jan het gat | <??met zand> | vrijwillig <met zand> | vulde. | |
that | Jan the hole | with sand | voluntarily | filled | ||
'that Jan voluntarily filled the hole with sand.' |
The examples in this subsection thus suggest that the unmarked order of process adverbials is as follows: volition > manner > instrument/means > domain.
The passive construction in (192b) clearly shows that passive door-phrases precede comitative met-phrases: inverting the order results in a severely degraded result.
a. | dat | Marie het artikel | met Jan | besprak. | |
that | Marie the article | with Jan | discussed | ||
'that Marie discussed the article with Jan.' |
b. | dat | het artikel | <door Marie> | met Jan <*door Marie> | besproken | werd. | |
that | the article | by Marie | with Jan | discussed | was | ||
'that the article was discussed with Jan by Marie.' |
Although it is not difficult to find agentive door-phrases to the left of manner adverbs, there is evidence that this order is the result of focus movement: example (193a) shows that the door-phrase must follow the manner adverb if the nominal complement of the preposition door is a weak pronoun. Since comitative met-PPs must follow agentive door-PPs, we expect by transitivity that they also follow manner adverbials in the unmarked order: example (193b) shows that this expectation is indeed borne our.
a. | dat | het gat | <door Jan/*ʼm> | zorgvuldig <door Jan/ʼm> | gevuld | werd. | |
that | the hole | by Jan/him | carefully | filled | was | ||
'that the hole was carefully filled by Jan/him.' |
b. | dat | Marie het probleem | <met Jan/*ʼm> | grondig <met Jan/ʼm> | besprak. | |
that | Marie the problem | with Jan/him | thoroughly | discussed | ||
'that Marie discussed the problem with Jan/him thoroughly.' |
Example (194a) shows that comitative met-PPs precede instrument/means adverbials in the unmarked order: inverting the order gives rise to a degraded result regardless of the form of the nominal complement of the preposition met. Since comitative met-PPs follow agentive door-PPs in the unmarked order, we expect by transitivity that door-phrases also precede instrument/means adverbials; example (194b) shows that this expectation is also borne out.
a. | dat | Jan het gat | met Marie/ʼr | met zand | vulde. | |
that | Jan the hole | with Marie | with sand | filled | ||
'that Jan filled the hole with sand with Marie/her.' |
b. | dat | het gat | door Jan/ʼm | met zand | gevuld | werd. | |
that | the hole | by Jan/him | with sand | filled | was | ||
'that the hole was filled with sand by Jan/him.' |
The examples in this subsection have established that in the unmarked case agentive adverbials are located between the manner and the instrument/means adverbials, while agentive door-PPs precede comitative met-PPs. We therefore conclude that the unmarked order of process and agentive adverbials is as follows: volition > manner > agentive > comitative > instrument/means > domain.
In the middle field of the clause, temporal VP adverbials precede locational VP adverbials, and they both seem most comfortable in a position preceding the manner adverbs, although it is not easy to show conclusively that this is their unmarked position.
a. | dat | Jan waarschijnlijk | om drie uur | in het park | gaat | wandelen. | |
that | Jan probably | at 3 o’clock | in the park | goes | walk | ||
'that Jan will probably go walking in the park at 3 oʼclock.' |
b. | dat | Jan waarschijnlijk | om drie uur | zachtjes | wegsluipt. | |
that | Jan probably | at 3 o’clock | quietly | away-slips | ||
'that Jan probably slips away quietly at 3 o'clock.' |
c. | dat | Jan waarschijnlijk | in het park | intensief | wil | trainen. | |
that | Jan probably | in the park | intensively | wants | train | ||
'that Jan probably wants to train intensively in the park.' |
It is also difficult to establish the unmarked order of the spatio-temporal and volitional adverbials such as vrijwillig ‘voluntarily’ and graag'gladly', as the latter can easily appear in the positions indicated by “✓” and only marginally appear in the position indicated by the question mark.
Jan gaat waarschijnlijk <✓> | om drie uur <✓> | in het park <?> | wandelen. | ||
Jan goes probably | at 3 o’clock | in the park | walk | ||
'Jan will probably go walking gladly in the park at three o'clock.' |
We assume provisionally that the volitional adverbs are base-generated above the temporal adverbials and that the alternate orders are derived by leftward movement of the spatio-temporal adverbials. If true, this gives rise to the following unmarked order of VP adverbials: volition > temporal > locational > manner > agentive > comitative > instrument/means > domain.
The examples in (197) show that contingency adverbials precede time adverbials; inverting this order gives rise to an infelicitous result. It is not easy to establish whether the various subtypes of contingency adverbs exhibit an unmarked order, as they do not easily co-occur; we will therefore not digress on this issue.
a. | dat | Jan waarschijnlijk | door de file | te laat | in Utrecht | zal | zijn. | |
that | Jan probably | by the traffic.jam | too late | in Utrecht | will | be | ||
'that Jan will probably be in Utrecht too late due to the traffic jam.' |
b. | dat | Jan waarschijnlijk | vanwege het vakantieverkeer | vroeg | vertrekt. | ||
that | Jan probably | because.of the holiday.traffic | early | leaves | |||
'that Jan will probably leave early because of the holiday traffic.' |
c. | dat | Jan waarschijnlijk | ondanks de file | op tijd | in Utrecht | zal | zijn. | |
that | Jan probably | despite the traffic.jam | in time | in Utrecht | will | be | ||
'that Jan will probably be in Utrecht in time despite the traffic jam.' |
Example (198) shows that the contingency adverbials also preferably precede the volition adverbials.
dat | de minister | <??vrijwillig> | vanwege het schandaal <vrijwillig> | aftrad. | ||
that | the minister | voluntarily | because.of the scandal | resigned | ||
'that the minister resigned voluntarily because of the scandal.' |
This means that so far we have established the following unmarked order of VP adverbials: contingency > volition > temporal > locational > manner > agentive > comitative > instrument/means > domain.
VP adverbials such as erg in (199) normally follow the locational VP adverbials. Although it is not difficult to find agentive door-phrases to the left of predicate-degree adverbials, there is reason for assuming that this is the result of focus movement: Example (199a) shows that the door-phrase must follow the degree adverbial if the nominal complement of the preposition door is a weak pronoun.
a. | Marie | wordt | waarschijnlijk | <??erg> | in Utrecht <erg> | bewonderd | |
Marie | is | probably | greatly | in Utrecht | admired | ||
'Marie is probably admired greatly in Utrecht.' |
b. | Marie wordt | <door Peter/*ʼm> | erg <door Peter/ʼm> | bewonderd. | |
Marie is | by Peter/him | greatly | admired | ||
'Marie is greatly admired by Peter/him.' |
Because manner and degree adverbials do not seem to co-occur, the examples in (199) make the picture complete by showing that the predicate-degree adverbs are located between the locational and the agentive adverbials in the unmarked case.
The discussion above has shown that VP adverbials exhibit the unmarked word order in (200). Since the relative order of VP adverbials has not received much attention in the literature so far, we have to leave it to future research to investigate whether this linear hierarchy can stand closer scrutiny.
Unmarked word order ofVP adverbials: contingency> volition > temporal > locational > manner/predicate-degree > agentive > comitative > instrument/means > domain.Unmarked word order ofVP adverbials: contingency> volition > temporal > locational > manner/predicate-degree > agentive > comitative > instrument/means > domain. |
This subsection discusses the unmarked word order of the set of clause adverbials in (201), which were also taken as our point of departure in Section 8.2.2. It should be pointed out that this set of clause adverbials is not identical to the set of adverbials that Cinque (1999) locates in the functional domain, as some of the these were shown to function as VP adverbials according to the adverbial tests introduced in Section 8.1; this holds, e.g., for volition adverbials like vrijwillig'voluntarily' and opzettelijk'deliberately'. The main conclusion of our discussion will be, however, that the unmarked order found in Dutch shows a considerable similarity to what is expected on the basis of the Cinque’s cross-linguistic structural hierarchy of adverbials in the functional domain of the clause. His structural, top-down order more or less coincides with the unmarked linear, left-right order in the middle field of the clause.
a. | Polarity: negation (niet'not' ); affirmation (wel) |
b. | Focus particles (alleen'only', ook'too', zelfs'even', etc.) |
c. | Aspectual: habitual; iterative; frequentative; continuative; etc. |
d. | Clause-degree (bijna'nearly'; amper'hardly', etc.) |
e. | Propositional modal (waarschijnlijk'probably', blijkbaar'apparently', etc.) |
f. | Subject-oriented (stom genoeg'stupidly', wijselijk'wisely', etc.) |
g. | Subjective: factive (e.g., helaas'unfortunately' ); non-factive |
h. | Point-of-view (volgens Els'according to Els' ) |
i. | Spatio-temporal: place; time |
j. | Contingency: cause; reason; condition; concession |
k. | Domain (juridisch gezien'legally', moreel gezien'morally', etc.) |
l. | Conjunctive (echter'however', derhalve'therefore', etc.) |
m. | Speech-act related (eerlijk gezegd'honestly' , etc.) |
In order to facilitate the discussion, the adverbials in (201) are already listed in the order that more or less reflects their unmarked linear order in the middle field of the clause, although it is not always easy to demonstrate this because of co-occurrence restrictions. For this reason, we restrict ourselves to a limited subset of clear cases; a more detailed discussion is not possible at this stage for want of sufficiently rich empirical research. We will also divide the clause adverbial types into several larger subgroups. Subsection A starts with the adverbials in (201a-e), which we will refer to as scope-bearing adverbials, as these can be seen as operators over the proposition expressed by the lexical domain of the verb. Subsection B discusses the adverbials in (201f-h), which we will refer to as evaluative adverbials as these are involved in providing a subjective evaluation of the proposition expressed by the clause. Subsection C addresses the spatio-temporal and the contingency adverbials in (201i&j) and Subsection D concludes with the remaining cases in (201k-m).
The polarity adverbials functions as the demarcations par excellence of the boundary between the lexical and the functional domain: in non-contrastive contexts, they are followed by the VP adverbials and preceded by the clause adverbials. We illustrate this in (202) for the comitative VP adverbial met ʼm'with him' and the epistemic clause adverbial waarschijnlijk'probably'.
dat | Marie waarschijnlijk | niet/wel | met ʼm | wil | spelen. | ||
that | Marie probably | not/aff | with him | wants | play | ||
'that Marie probably wants/doesnʼt want to play with him.' |
Note in passing that there are robust reasons for assuming that at least the negative adverb niet is located in the specifier position of a functional projection NegP, which may also be the landing site of larger negative phrases in the clause; if so, it shows clearly that negation itself is part of the functional domain of the clause. We will not digress on this here but refer the reader to Section 13.3.1 for extensive discussion.
Example (203a) illustrates that focus particles such as ook'also' are placed between the epistemic modals and the polarity adverbials. Example (203b) shows that contrastively focused phrases may occupy the same position as focus particles; for this reason, Section 13.3.2 argues that focus particles are part of a functional projection FocP. Note in passing that the negative adverb niet can easily substitute for affirmative wel in these examples.
a. | dat | Marie waarschijnlijk | ook | wel | met ʼm | wil | spelen. | |
that | Marie probably | also | aff | with him | wants | play | ||
'that Marie probably also wants to play with him.' |
b. | dat | Marie waarschijnlijk | ook met hem | wel | wil | spelen. | |
that | Marie probably | also with him | aff | wants | play | ||
'that Marie probably also wants to play with him.' |
Aspectual adverbials precede the focus particles but follow the modal epistemic modals. We illustrate this for the habitual adverbial gewoonlijk'usually'; example (204a) shows that it must precede the focus particle ook, while the slightly awkward example in (204b) shows that it must follow the epistemic modal waarschijnlijk.
a. | dat | Marie gewoonlijk | ook | wel | met ʼm | wil | spelen. | |
that | Marie usually | also | aff | with him | wants | play | ||
'that Marie usually does want to play with him as well.' |
b. | dat | Marie waarschijnlijk | gewoonlijk | wel | met ʼm | wil | spelen. | |
that | Marie probably | usually | aff | with him | wants | play | ||
'that probably Marie usually does wants to play with him.' |
Example (205a) shows that the clause-degree adverbial bijna can precede focus particles such as ook, but that it is not entirely impossible to have it after the focus particles. In many cases the second order is fully acceptable but this may be due to the fact that bijna can also be used as a non-clausal modifier; cf. bijna leeg'nearly empty'. The somewhat awkward construction in example (205b) shows that clause-degree adverbials follow the epistemic modals.
a. | dat | Marie | <bijna> | ook <?bijna> | met ʼm | ging | spelen. | |
that | Marie | nearly | also | with him | went | play | ||
'that Marie nearly started to play with him as well.' |
b. | dat | Marie waarschijnlijk | bijna | ook | met ʼm | ging | spelen. | |
that | Marie probably | nearly | also | with him | went | play | ||
'that Marie probably nearly also started to play with him.' |
We conclude from the examples in (205) that clause-degree adverbials are located in between the epistemic modals and the focus particles but it is clear that this must be a preliminary conclusion: more research is needed to establish this more firmly.
Above it was already shown for the epistemic modals that propositional-modal adverbials precede negation, focal particles, frequentative adverbial and clause-degree adverbials. The fact that the epistemic modal waarschijnlijk'probably' in the examples given above can easily be replaced by blijkbaar'evidently' shows that this also holds for evidential modals. Since the epistemic and evidential modal adverbials do not easily co-occur, we will not speculate on their relative order.
The discussion above has shown that scope-bearing clause adverbials exhibit the unmarked word order in (206). Because relatively little research on Dutch has been done in this area, our conclusions should be considered provisional.
Unmarked word order of scope-bearing clause adverbials: propositional modal > clause-degree > aspectual > focus > negation |
The placement of subject-oriented adverbials such as wijselijk'wisely' with respect to the scope adverbials discussed in the previous subsection is not entirely clear. Example (207a) first provides a clear example showing that speaker-oriented adverbials must precede focus particles and negation; the asterisks indicate positions in which subject-oriented adverbials cannot occur. Example (207b) shows that subject-oriented adverbials can easily precede aspectual adverbials such as habitual gewoonlijk, but placing them after gewoonlijk is at least marginally possible. The slightly awkward (c)-examples, finally, show that for some speakers the relative order of the subject-oriented and propositional adverbials is essentially free; judgments seem to vary from speaker to speaker and from instance to instance.
a. | dat | Marie | <wijselijk> | ook <*> | niet <*> | met ʼm | wil | spelen. | |
that | Marie | wisely | also | not | with him | wants | play | ||
'that Marie wisely doesnʼt want to play with him either.' |
b. | dat | Marie | <wijselijk> | gewoonlijk <?wijselijk> | niet | met ʼm | wil spelen. | |
that | Marie | wisely | usually | not | with him | wants play | ||
'that wisely Marie normally/often doesnʼt want to play with him.' |
c. | dat | Marie wijselijk | waarschijnlijk <#> | niet | met ʼm | wil | spelen. | |
that | Marie wisely | probably | not | with him | wants | play | ||
'that wisely Marie probably doesnʼt want to play with him.' |
c'. | dat | Marie wijselijk | blijkbaar <#> | niet | met ʼm | wil | spelen. | |
that | Marie wisely | evidently | not | with him | wants | play | ||
'that wisely Marie apparently doesnʼt want to play with him.' |
We provisionally conclude from (207) that speaker-oriented adverbials precede all scope adverbials with the exception of the propositional modal adverbials. That their ordering vis-a-vis propositional modals is not very strict may be related to the fact that at least the epistemic modals are also evaluative, in the sense that they too provide an assessment of the state-of-affairs expressed by the clause.
Subjective adverbials like gelukkig'fortunately' and helaas'unfortunately' are factive in the sense that they imply that the proposition is true; this accounts for the fact illustrated in (208) that they always give rise to an awkward result in combination with propositional adverbials, as these crucially do not presuppose the truth of the proposition.
a. | $ | dat | Jan | <gelukkig> | waarschijnlijk <gelukkig> | vertrekt. |
that | Jan | fortunately | probably | leaves |
b. | $ | dat | Jan | <helaas> | waarschijnlijk <helaas > | vertrekt. |
that | Jan | unfortunately | probably | leaves |
Example (209) shows that non-factive subjective adverbials such as naar ik vrees'as I fear' must precede the propositional modals such as waarschijnlijk'probably' (unless naar ik vrees is preceded and followed by an intonation break, in which case we are dealing with an epenthetic construction). We therefore conclude that the subjective adverbials precede the propositional adverbials in the unmarked order.
dat | Marie naar ik vrees | waarschijnlijk | niet | met ʼm | wil | spelen. | ||
that | Marie as I fear | probably | not | with him | wants | play | ||
'that I fear that Marie probably doesn't want to play with him.' |
Subjective adverbials and epistemic modals provide an assessment of the state-of-affairs referred to by the sentence. The default interpretation is that the assessment is the speaker’s but this interpretation can easily be overridden by contextual information. One way of doing this is by using a point-of-view adverbial such as volgens Els'according to Els'; cf. Section 8.2.2, sub VIII. The examples in (210) show that such adverbials precede the subjective and epistemic modal adverbials: this might be a matter of scope, given that the interpretation of the latter depends on the former, but this is probably not the full story because subsection C will show that they also precede spatio-temporal and contingency adverbials.
a. | Jan komt | <volgens Els> | zeker <??volgens Els> | op visite. | epistemic | |
Jan comes | according.to Els | certainly | on visit |
b. | Jan bleef | <volgens Els> | wijselijk <??volgens Els> | thuis. | subject-oriented | |
Jan stayed | according.to Els | wisely | at.home |
c. | Jan is | <volgens Els> | gelukkig <??volgens Els> | ontslagen. | subjective | |
Jan is | according.to Els | fortunately | fired |
The discussion in this subsection has shown that we can extend the word-order generalization in (206) to the one in (211). Our conclusions should again be considered as preliminary, for the reason indicated in the previous subsection.
Unmarked word order of scope-bearing and evaluative clause adverbials: point-of-view > subjective > subject-oriented/propositional modal > clause-degree > aspectual > focus > negationUnmarked word order of scope-bearing and evaluative clause adverbials: point-of-view > subjective > subject-oriented/propositional modal > clause-degree > aspectual > focus > negation |
The examples in (212) show that clausal spatio-temporal adverbials can easily precede the propositional modals. That spatio-temporal adverbials cannot follow the propositional adverbials is sometimes difficult to demonstrate because the resulting strings are often acceptable under an alternative analysis: for instance, morgenvroeg in dat Jan waarschijnlijk morgenvroeg vertrekt'that Jan will probably leave early tomorrow' clearly functions as a one-word VP adverbial. We refer to Section 8.2 for an extensive discussion on determining the actual status of spatio-temporal adverbials as VP or as clause adverbials.
a. | dat | Jan morgen | waarschijnlijk | vroeg | vertrekt. | |
that | Jan tomorrow | probably | early | leaves | ||
'that Jan will probably leave early tomorrow.' |
b. | dat | Jan in Utrechtwaarschijnlijk | bij zijn tante | logeert. | |
that | Jan in Utrecht probably | with his aunt | stays | ||
'that Jan will probably stay with his aunt in Utrecht.' |
The examples in (213) show that clausal spatio-temporal adverbials can also precede subject-oriented adverbs such as wijselijk'wisely' and subjective adverbials such as helaas'unfortunately', although the reverse order seems at least marginally possible, too.
a. | dat | Jan <morgen> | helaas/wijselijk <(?)morgen> | niet | komt. | |
that | Jan tomorrow | unfortunately/wisely | not | comes | ||
'that Jan unfortunately/wisely wonʼt come tomorrow.' |
b. | dat | Jan <in Utrecht> | helaas/wijselijk <(?)in Utrecht> | niet | overnacht. | |
that | Jan in Utrecht | unfortunately/wisely | not | stays.overnight | ||
'that Jan unfortunately/wisely wonʼt spend the night in Utrecht.' |
Point-of-view adverbials such as volgens Els'according to Els', on the other hand, preferably precede the spatio-temporal adverbials; this illustrated in (214).
a. | dat | Jan | <??morgen> | volgens Els <morgen> | niet komt. | |
that | Jan | tomorrow | according.to Els | not comes | ||
'that according to Els Jan wonʼt come tomorrow.' |
b. | dat | Jan | <??in Utrecht> | volgens Els <in U.> | waarschijnlijk | overnacht. | |
that | Jan | in Utrecht | according.to Els | probably | stays.overnight | ||
'that according to Els Jan will probably spend the night in Utrecht.' |
Contingency adverbials can precede or follow the clausal spatio-temporal adverbials; we illustrate this in (215) for the reason adverbial wegens ziekte'because of illness' only. It seems that the order in which they precede the spatio-temporal adverbials is somewhat more natural but the contrast is not sharp, so we will leave it for later to determine the unmarked order more precisely. Example (215c) further shows that contingency adverbials prefer to precede subject-oriented adverbials.
a. | dat | Els | <morgen> | vanwege ziekte <morgen> | waarschijnlijk | niet | zingt. | |
that | Els | tomorrow | because.of illness | probably | not | sings | ||
'that Els probably wonʼt sing tomorrow because of illness.' |
b. | dat | Els | <in Utrecht> | vanwege ziekte <in U.> | waarschijnlijk | niet | zingt. | |
that | Els | in Utrecht | because.of illness | probably | not | sings | ||
'that Els probably wonʼt sing in Utrecht because of illness.' |
c. | dat | Els | morgen | <??wijselijk> | vanwege ziekte <wijselijk> | niet | zingt. | |
that | Els | tomorrow | wisely | because .of illness | not | sings | ||
'that Els wisely wonʼt sing tomorrow because of illness.' |
We provisionally conclude on the basis of the examples in this subsection that the spatio-temporal and contingency adverbials are located between the point-of-view and the subjective adverbials, although there is still unclarity about the unmarked order of the spatio-temporal and the subjective/subject-oriented adverbials.
Unmarked word order of clause adverbials: point-of-view > contingency/spatio-temporal > subjective > subject-oriented/ propositional modal > clause-degree > aspectual > focus > negationUnmarked word order of clause adverbials: point-of-view > contingency/spatio-temporal > subjective > subject-oriented/ propositional modal > clause-degree > aspectual > focus > negation |
Domain adverbials such as juridisch gezien'legally speaking/from a legal point of view' in (217) are relatively high in the functional domain in the clause. Because they restrict the application of the complete clause, there is a strong tendency to place them in sentence-initial position, but they may also occur in the middle field of the clause.
a. | Juridisch gezien | heeft | Jan waarschijnlijk | gelijk. | |
legally seen | has | Jan probably | right | ||
'Legally speaking, Jan is probably correct.' |
b. | Jan heeft | juridisch gezien | waarschijnlijk | gelijk. | |
Jan has | legally seen | probably | right |
Something similar holds for speech-act related adverbials such as eerlijk gezegd'honestly speaking' in (218). Because they comment on the speech act as a whole, there is a strong tendency to place them in sentence-initial position but, again, they may occur in the middle field of the clause.
a. | Eerlijk gezegd | kan | ik | het | niet | geloven. | |
honestly spoken | can | I | it | not | believe | ||
'Honestly speaking, I cannot believe it.' |
b. | Ik | kan | het | eerlijk gezegd | niet | geloven. | |
I | can | it | honestly spoken | not | believe |
It is, however, not easy to determine their unmarked position in the middle field of the clause more precisely: the examples in (219) show, for instance, that the domain and speech-act related adverbials can be placed before or after the clausal temporal adverbials. Judgments seem to differ from case to case and person to person, and both orders can be found on the internet.
a. | Jan had | <juridisch gezien> | gisteren <juridisch gezien> | gelijk. | |
Jan had | legally seen | yesterday | right | ||
'Legally speaking, Jan was right yesterday.' |
b. | Ik | kon | het | <eerlijk gezegd> | gisteren <eerlijk gezegd> | niet | geloven. | |
I | could | it | honestly spoken | yesterday | not | believe | ||
'Honestly speaking, I couldnʼt believe it yesterday.' |
An additional problem for determining the unmarked position of domain and speech-act adverbials more precisely is that they often occur as parentheticals. This is especially clear for the speech-act adverbial eerlijk gezegd, as the examples in (220) show that it may also precede the first position of the sentence or be placed in clause-final position; the comma’s indicate an intonation break.
a. | Eerlijk gezegd, | ik | kan | het | niet | geloven. | |
honestly spoken | I | can | it | not | believe |
b. | Ik | kan | het | niet | geloven, | eerlijk gezegd. | |
I | can | it | not | believe | honestly spoken |
Similar problems arise for conjunctive adverbials such as echter'however' in (221), which can be used at various positions in the sentence. The options available seem to differ from case to case.
a. | Echter, | Jan zal | morgen | waarschijnlijk | vroeg | vertrekken. | |
however | Jan will | tomorrow | probably | early | leave | ||
'However, Jan will probably leave early tomorrow.' |
b. | Jan, echter, zal morgen waarschijnlijk vroeg vertrekken. |
c. | Jan zal echter morgen waarschijnlijk vroeg vertrekken. |
d. | Jan zal | morgen echter waarschijnlijk vroeg vertrekken. |
Because the word order problems pointed out above have not yet been investigated in greater depth, it seems premature to speculate on the precise unmarked position of these adverbials: we can only conclude that that they are relatively high in the linear hierarchy in (216).
This section has discussed the unmarked order of adverbial phrases. In order to eliminate the effects of movement as much as possible we restricted our attention to the order of adverbials in the middle field of the clause. Furthermore, we excluded sentences with contrastive accent and adverbial proforms. Our investigation has shown that both the VP adverbials as well as the clause adverbials are subject to ordering restrictions. The two linear hierarchies in (222) summarize our findings. We did not include the domain, speech-act related and conjunctive adverbials in these hierarchies: although it is clear that they are located high up in the hierarchy in (222a), it is difficult for various reasons to locate them more precisely.
a. | Clause adverbials: point-of-view > contingency/spatio-temporal > subjective > subject-oriented/ propositional modal > clause-degree > aspectual > focus > negationClause adverbials: point-of-view > contingency/spatio-temporal > subjective > subject-oriented/ propositional modal > clause-degree > aspectual > focus > negation |
b. | VP adverbials: contingency > volition > temporal > locational > manner/predicate-degree > agentive > comitative > instrument/means > domain.VP adverbials: contingency > volition > temporal > locational > manner/predicate-degree > agentive > comitative > instrument/means > domain. |
Because the ordering of clause adverbials has not been studied in very great detail so far in the literature on Dutch, the proposed ordering should be considered preliminary, pending further investigation. Cinque’s (1999) typological work suggests, for example, that (222a) can be fine-tuned by adding more fine-grained distinctions. Other problems complicating the investigation are the (semantic) co-occurrence restrictions we occasionally find as well as the fact that sometimes more than one linear order is fully acceptable.
- 1999Adverbs and functional heads: a cross-linguistic perspective.Oxford studies in comparative syntax.
- 1999Adverbs and functional heads: a cross-linguistic perspective.Oxford studies in comparative syntax.
- 1999Adverbs and functional heads: a cross-linguistic perspective.Oxford studies in comparative syntax.
- 2006Complement and Adverbial PPs: Implications for Clause StructureRestructuring and Functional HeadsOxford/New YorkOxford University Press
- 1974Het werkwoord als spiegelcentrumSpektator3601-618
- 2005The order of prepositional phrases in the structure of the clauseAmsterdam/PhiladelphiaJohn Benjamins